Skip to main content

Atypical early neural responses to native and non-native language in infants at high likelihood for developing autism

Abstract

Background

Language difficulties are common in autism spectrum disorder (ASD), a neurodevelopmental condition characterized by impairments in social communication as well as restricted and repetitive behaviors. Amongst infant siblings of children with an ASD diagnosis – who are at higher likelihood for developing ASD – a high proportion also show difficulties and delays in language acquisition.

Methods

In this study, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine differences in language processing in 9-month-old infants at high (HL) and typical (TL) familial likelihood for ASD. Infants were presented with native (English) and novel (Japanese) speech while sleeping naturally in the scanner. Whole-brain and a priori region-of-interest analyses were conducted to evaluate neural differences in language processing based on likelihood group and language condition.

Results

HL infants showed attenuated responses to speech in general, particularly in left temporal language areas, as well as a lack of neural discrimination between the native and novel languages compared to the TL group. Importantly, we also demonstrate that HL infants show distinctly atypical patterns of lateralization for speech processing, particularly during native speech processing, suggesting a failure to left-lateralize. Limitations: The sample size, particularly for the TL group, is relatively modest because of the challenges inherent to collecting auditory stimulus-evoked data from sleeping participants, as well as retention and follow-up difficulties posed by the COVID-19 pandemic. The groups were not matched on some demographic variables, but the present findings held even after accounting for these differences.

Conclusions

To our knowledge, this is the first fMRI study to directly measure autism-associated atypicalities in native language uptake during infancy. These findings provide a better understanding of the neurodevelopmental underpinnings of language delay in ASD, which is a prerequisite step for developing earlier and more effective interventions for autistic children and HL siblings who experience language impairments.

Background

Language difficulties, including speech delays and impairments in receptive language, are the most commonly reported first signs of autism spectrum disorder [1, 2] (ASD), a neurodevelopmental condition that affects approximately one in 36 children in the United States [3]. Indeed, communication difficulties are part of the core criteria for autism and approximately 25% of autistic individuals are minimally verbal or entirely nonverbal [4]. These prevalent, and often profound, impairments underscore the need to understand how early brain development contributes to atypical language outcomes in autism. Due to the vast heterogeneity that characterizes the onset, developmental course, and severity of core ASD symptoms [5], studies searching for early-life biomarkers of autism have continued to investigate how early atypical development cascades into complex symptom profiles.

Humans begin to acquire language before birth, a process that involves many months of uptake of the prosodic contours – such as rhythm, stress, and pitch – of the language spoken ex utero [6, 7]. Soon after birth, this uptake is displayed when neonates show a preference for their native language over a novel language [8]. In mid- to late infancy, this familiarity preference is then supplanted by a preference for novel stimuli [9], which roughly coincides with the process of phonemic “perceptual narrowing,” in which infants lose the ability to discriminate between nonnative sounds between six and 10 months of age [10, 11]. As an inherently social process, language acquisition depends heavily on joint attention and affect sharing skills, both of which are perturbed in ASD [12, 13]. Indeed, language impairments associated with autism, such as a failure for infants to orient to their own name [14], can be detected well before a clinical ASD diagnosis is obtained. A recent review of pre-speech milestones in infants later diagnosed with ASD [15] summarized the behavioral atypicalities that can be found across the first year of life. These included lower rates of vocalizing [16], vocalizations with simpler intonations and acoustic contours [17], and, in some cases, a failure to coo at developmentally appropriate ages [18]. By approximately 12 months of age, infants later diagnosed with ASD also exhibit lower rates of canonical babbling [19, 20], an important speech milestone.

Despite these promising behavioral markers, infants who later receive an ASD diagnosis cannot yet be reliably identified within the first year of life with current linguistic assessment tools. Although ASD can be diagnosed as early as 18 months of age [21], the median age of diagnosis in the United States lags behind, at an average of three to five years [22, 23]. Closing this diagnostic gap is critical for children to receive earlier access to interventions, which is known to improve linguistic and cognitive outcomes [24]. Moreover, understanding the considerable variability in the severity and developmental course of language impairments in ASD will help inform more individualized interventions for affected children.

Consistent with the behavioral literature in infancy, pre-partum studies have demonstrated that typically developing fetuses show increased heartrate, a physiological response associated with preferential attention, when presented with a novel language [6]. Likewise, fetuses also show increased heartrate [6] and activation in temporal language areas when hearing their mother’s voice compared to a stranger’s voice [7]. Further, activity in frontal and temporal regions – associated with language processing in adults – has been demonstrated in two-day-old neonates passively exposed to speech [25]. Collectively, these studies not only show that native language acquisition begins in the earliest stages of human development, but that the early antecedents of the adult language network [26] are already in place.

Atypicalities in the brain’s responses to speech have been broadly documented in toddlers and young children with autism. Canonical activity in temporal language areas is notably diminished in autistic toddlers [27], particularly those with delayed language [28]. Moreover, responses to speech, which are left-lateralized in most individuals, are atypically right-lateralized in toddlers with ASD [29]. Some of these differences may stem from early deficits in attention to socially relevant and/or affective speech, such as infant-directed speech. Indeed, recent work shows that autistic toddlers with poor language skills show weaker brain responses in temporal cortices when hearing infant-directed speech [27], providing a neural correlate for the diminished attention to early affective speech – and language in general – seen in ASD. Additionally, toddlers with autism show reduced left-hemisphere specialization for language processing compared to controls [29, 30], with atypical rightward lateralization of language-related white matter tracts detected in infants as young as six weeks [31]. Newer evidence also indicates that the cerebellum (known to be involved in language processing [32]) may play a role in language delays associated with ASD. Altered connectivity between the right crus I of the cerebellum – often implicated in language function [33] – and the cortex has been observed in language-delayed infants who also later developed ASD and/or other developmental concerns [34].

A growing body of work has focused on infancy, before autism can be reliably diagnosed, by prospectively studying infants at high likelihood (HL) for ASD based on family history. HL infants, who have an older sibling with a confirmed diagnosis, have a one in five chance of receiving an ASD diagnosis by age three [35]. HL infants have shown reduced neural specialization for language [36], among other atypical brain responses [37], during passive exposure to speech. In another paradigm that examined implicit language learning, HL infants, particularly those with greater ASD symptoms, showed reductions in neural activity associated with statistical language learning at nine months of age [38]. These studies show that passive activation paradigms can consistently uncover the neural traces of language atypicalities in infants, including differences associated with autism likelihood and later symptoms. However, prior studies of speech processing in infants have either compared the neural processing of native language against that of non-speech sounds or backwards speech. To date, no studies have directly compared neural responses to native versus non-native language in HL infants using fMRI, even though doing so could provide an index of native language uptake at a very early age. Indeed, a few studies using event-related electroencephalography have found atypicalities in native-nonnative phoneme processing in HL infants (e.g., [39]; for a review, see [40]).

In this study, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to index early atypicalities in native language acquisition that may be related to ASD. Specifically, we compared infant brain responses to native versus non-native speech at nine months of age, when infants are expected to show a novelty preference for a new language, in infants at varying likelihood for ASD: HL infants and “typical likelihood” (TL) controls. Based on prior work [29, 36], we expected that the HL group would be characterized by diminished responses to speech stimuli in language networks, including the cerebellum, with reduced neural differentiation between native and non-native languages. Furthermore, we expected the HL group to show atypically lateralized processing of native speech, characterized by more bilateral language processing, or even a rightward bias, given evidence that right-lateralized language processing may be compensatory in older autistic individuals [41].

Materials & methods

Participants

Participants were enrolled as part of a longitudinal study of behavioral and neural markers of ASD as part of UCLA’s Autism Center for Excellence. Based on family history of ASD, infants were assigned to the following groups: “High Likelihood” (HL) infants had at least one older sibling with a confirmed clinical diagnosis of autism, and “Typical Likelihood” (TL) infants had no first or second-degree family members with ASD or another known neurodevelopmental disorder. For both groups, exclusionary criteria included: (1) genetic or neurological conditions associated with ASD (e.g., fragile X syndrome, tuberous sclerosis), (2) chronic developmental condition or perinatal insult, (3) severe visual, hearing, or motor impairment, and (4) contraindication for MRI. Informed consent was obtained from participants’ parents or legal guardians, and all study protocols were approved by UCLA’s Institutional Review Board.

For this particular study, participants were required to have at-home English exposure of at least 50%, with 0% exposure to Japanese. Although some infants may be acquiring multiple “first” languages, this ensured that English could be considered the native language of all participants, with Japanese remaining entirely novel. In cases where sibling pairs were recruited, we retained only one sibling (the sibling with lowest head motion during fMRI and/or with the most behavioral data available) in order to preserve independence of observations. These criteria yielded a sample of 63 infants (38 HL, 25 TL). Out of these, we excluded two TL participants due to scanner artifacts, two HL participants due to motion artifacts, and 10 participants (six HL, four TL) due to lack of any response in primary auditory cortices even at extremely liberal thresholds (Z > 1.1, uncorrected) to guard against the possibility of a stimulus presentation failure. Therefore, the final imaging sample consisted of 49 infants (30 HL, 19 TL). The HL and TL groups did not significantly differ on age at scan, race, ethnicity, exposure to English, or head motion during the MRI scan, but the groups significantly differed on sex, maternal education, and birth order (Table 1). As these variables can affect language development, post-hoc linear regressions were conducted to ensure that group differences held when controlling for these potential confounds (see Supplementary Information).

Behavioral measures

Several behavioral assessments were administered at 12 and 36 months of age to evaluate the development of social and language skills, as well as ASD symptomatology. The Mullen Scales of Early Learning [42] (MSEL) were administered at 12 and 36 months of age to index five components of early development: receptive and expressive language, fine and gross motor function, and visual reception. Together, these subscales (excluding gross motor) are combined into an Early Learning Composite (ELC) score. The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition [43] (VABS-II), a parental interview measure that assesses four domains including communication, daily living, socialization, and motor skills, was administered at 12 and 36 months. Here, we focused on the communication and socialization subscales. Early lexical development was indexed using the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory [44] (MCDI), tracking the number of words produced and comprehended at 12 months, and the number of words produced at 36 months. The Early Social Communication Scales [45] (ESCS), a play-based, structured assessment of nonverbal social communication, was administered at 12 months to measure the rate per minute of responding to joint attention (RJA). Early signs of autism symptomatology were assessed using the Autism Observation Scale for Infants [46] (AOSI) at 12 months, with the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-2nd Edition [47] (ADOS-2) collected at 36 months. At the 36-month behavioral visit, 26 HL and 16 TL participants underwent a clinical assessment to determine outcome classification: Typically Developing, Autism Spectrum Disorder, or Other Concerns (speech/language delay, subclinical ASD symptoms, or other developmental delays as assessed by a licensed UCLA clinician).

Table 1 Participant demographics, behavioral assessments, and outcome assessments by likelihood group. Count is shown for categorical variables. Mean ± standard deviation is shown for continuous variables (age, motion metrics, and behavioral assessments)

Sample sizes for behavioral data: MSEL at 12 months: 18 TL, 26 HL; Vineland at 12 months: 12 TL, 23 HL; MCDI at 12 months: 17 TL, 25 HL; AOSI at 12 months: 15 TL, 27 HL; ESCS at 12 months: 14 TL, 22 HL; MSEL at 36 months: 15 TL, 26 HL; Vineland at 36 months: 16 TL, 24 HL; MCDI at 36 months: 15 TL, 21 HL; ADOS-2 at 36 months: 13 TL, 19 HL.

Acronyms: HL: high likelihood for autism; TL: typical likelihood for autism; MSEL: Mullen Scales of Early Learning; AOSI: Autism Observation Scale for Infants; ADOS-2: Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition; ASD: autism spectrum disorder; TD: typically developing; MCDI: MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories; ESCS: Early Social Communication Scales; IJA: initiating joint attention; RJA: responding to joint attention.

fMRI paradigm

Following a traditional block design, speech stimuli were presented in alternating segments of native (English) and novel (Japanese) speech interspersed with 12 s of silence, during natural sleep. Each language was presented eight times throughout the functional scan, for a total of 2.4 min of exposure to each language condition. These stimuli, previously used in behavioral studies of native language preference in infants [48], were recorded by different female native speakers of each language and were matched on important acoustic features, including duration, pitch, pitch range, intensity, and peak amplitude to ensure that discrimination was based solely upon the prosodic, rhythmic, and phonological differences between English and Japanese. The recordings were delivered via Optoacoustics MRI-compatible headphones, which also served to dampen ambient scanner noise. The order of stimulus presentation (i.e., English presented first versus Japanese presented first) was balanced between likelihood groups.

MRI Data Acquisition

MRI data were collected while infant participants were sleeping naturally, either on a Siemens Trio scanner (12-channel head coil) or, after an upgrade to the scanning facilities, a Siemens Prisma scanner (32-channel head coil). Scanner was therefore included as a nuisance covariate in group-level analyses. The data collection approach was based on recommended guidelines for neuroimaging in young infants [49, 50]. A scout localizing scan was used for slice prescription. Matched bandwidth T2-weighted high-resolution echo planar images were acquired co-planar to the functional scans to ensure identical distortion characteristics to the fMRI scans: TR = 5000ms, TE = 34ms, matrix size = 128 × 128, FOV = 192 mm, 34 slices, 1.5 mm in-plane resolution, with 4 mm-thick axial slices. The native language preference paradigm was acquired with a T2*-weighted functional sequence: TR = 3000ms, TE = 28ms, matrix size = 64 × 64, FOV = 192 mm, 34 slices, 3 mm in-plane resolution, with 4 mm-thick axial slices.

MRI visits were conducted in the evening. To help participants sleep, parents were encouraged to follow the infant’s bedtime routine prior to the scan and to swaddle and rock their child to sleep. Once asleep, infants were then transferred to the scanner bed, which was padded with linens and cushions. Silicone earplugs specially designed for infants and MiniMuffs Neonatal Noise Attenuators (Natus Medical, Inc., San Carlos, CA) were used for hearing protection underneath the stimulus presentation headphones. Noise suppression was not used. Infants were secured to the bed with a Velcro strap underneath a weighted blanket, and a member of the study staff remained in the scanner room with the infant at all times to monitor for signs of movement, wakefulness, or distress.

fMRI preprocessing

MRI data were preprocessed using FMRIB’s Software Library version 5.0.11 [51]. Functional data underwent rigid-body motion correction, while structural data underwent skull stripping with manual correction using FSL’s Brain Extraction Tool. Functional data were co-registered to the infant’s own high-resolution T2-weighted anatomical scan before being registered to a 1-year standard template [52] as in previous studies from our lab [38, 53, 54]. Registration at both steps was performed with a 12-parameter affine transformation, and registration was manually inspected for quality assurance. Data were spatially smoothed using a 6-mm Gaussian kernel and underwent 4D mean intensity normalization.

fMRI analysis

FSL FEAT was used to model BOLD responses to each condition of interest with respect to baseline, as well as to each other (English > Japanese, and Japanese > English), at the single-subject level. Motion-contaminated volumes, identified as outliers on FSL’s dvars metric (ΔRMS intensity > 50), were statistically censored from the analyses at the single-subject level [55]. Group-level analyses were conducted with a mixed effects linear model in FSL FEAT (FLAME 1 + 2) to generate group activation maps in common space, and to conduct contrasts between likelihood groups, languages, and their interaction. Although the proportion of participants who underwent MRI on each scanner did not differ between the two groups (χ2 = 0.224, p = 0.636; Trio: 16 HL, 11 TL; Prisma: 14 HL, eight TL) a nuisance covariate for the effect of scanner was also included in the group-level model. Within- and between-group contrasts in response to English and Japanese were statistically thresholded at Z > 2.3, cluster-corrected for multiple comparisons (P < 0.05, based on Gaussian random field theory). Permutation testing was not performed, given that FLAME performs comparably to permutation testing at Z > 2.3 [56]. However, we deemed significant and report only cluster peaks that surpass a threshold of Z > 3.1 (Table 2). Between-group and between-language contrasts were masked by clusters that displayed significant activation to either language condition, in either group based on the within-group activation maps.

ROI and laterality analyses

To hone in on group differences in canonical language regions and to examine language lateralization, we complemented the whole-brain analytical approach by employing a more targeted region-of-interest (ROI) analysis using brain parcellations derived from the standard atlas [50]. We selected a priori regions of interest (ROIs) canonically associated with language processing [26, 57] (See Figure S1 for selected ROIs), including the superior and middle temporal gyri, as well as the inferior frontal gyrus. In light of the growing evidence of the cerebellum’s role in language [32], we also considered two regions of the cerebellum known to be involved in language processing [57, 58]: crus I and lobule VI. Cerebellum parcellations were derived from the M-CRIB atlas [59] and warped to standard space. Activity in these ROIs was examined using two different metrics. First, to quantify activation strength for each ROI, for each infant we extracted parameter estimates from all voxels showing significant activation in either group and in either language. Second, the spatial extent of significant activation, quantified by the number of voxels with significant activation, was examined within our selected ROIs. This metric was calculated as the proportion of significant voxels within each ROI, extracted from each participant’s single-subject level data (thresholded at Z > 1.7 to account for individual differences in strength of activation). Mixed analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) were then used to model these two metrics (activation strength and cluster size) with likelihood group as a between-group predictor, ROI, language condition and hemisphere as within-group predictors, and scanner as a covariate of no interest.

Furthermore, given that language processing has been found to be atypically lateralized in toddlers with ASD [29, 30], these two metrics were used to interrogate the role of laterality more directly. A laterality index was calculated for each ROI using a standard laterality formula defined as the difference in values in the left and right hemispheres, divided by the sum of values in both left and right hemispheres: (L – R) / (L + R). The laterality indices of activation strength and cluster size were then examined as outcome variables in mixed ANCOVAs, which contained likelihood group as a between-group predictor, language condition and ROI as within-group predictors, and scanner as a covariate of no interest.

Results

Behavioral results

For the infants who provided neuroimaging data, at 12 months of age the TL group had significantly higher scores, compared to the HL group, on the MSEL receptive language (p = 0.004) and fine motor (p = 0.004) subscales, as well as higher rates of responding to joint attention (p < 0.001) and the VABS-II communication subscale (p = 0.02). At 12 months, there were no group differences for the expressive language, gross motor, and visual reception MSEL subscales, nor on the number of words understood or produced as measured by the MCDI. By 36 months, the TL group scored significantly higher than the HL group on both the MSEL receptive and expressive language subscales (p < 0.001 and p = 0.002, respectively), as well as the visual reception subscale (p = 0.02), but not the fine motor subscale. The TL group also had higher scores on the VABS-II communication (p < 0.001) and socialization subscales (p < 0.001). HL infants trended higher in ASD symptomatology (p = 0.08) as measured by the AOSI at 12 months, but there were no significant group differences in ASD symptomatology as measured by the ADOS-2 at 36 months. By 36 months, six HL infants received an ASD diagnosis, compared to none in the TL group (χ2 = 3.78, p = 0.05). We also found significantly higher rates of both ASD diagnosis and Other Concerns (language delay, intellectual delay, etc.) among the HL group (χ2 = 8.76, p = 0.013). Full demographic and behavioral data are reported in Table 1.

We also evaluated the effects of birth order, sex, maternal education, and second language exposure on language measures. We found that first-born infants had higher scores on the MSEL receptive (p = 0.02) and expressive (p = 0.001) language subscales at 36 months of age, as well as the Vineland communication (p = 0.005) and socialization (p = 0.01) subscales at 36 months. Female infants had higher scores on the MSEL fine motor (p < 0.001) and expressive language (p = 0.02) subscales at 36 months, as well as the Vineland communication (p = 0.003) and socialization (p < 0.001) subscales. Infants whose mothers had higher educational attainment had higher scores on the MSEL receptive language subscale (p = 0.04) at 36 months, as well as the Vineland communication (p = 0.007) and socialization (p = 0.02) subscale at 36 months. No associations were detected between the presence of second-language exposure (< 50%) and scores on language assessments at 12 or 36 months of age. Full statistics are available in Table S3. In general, these findings are consistent with prior research demonstrating more advanced language development for females [60], as well as first-borns and those raised by parents with higher socioeconomic status [61].

Neuroimaging Group results

At nine months of age, both groups of infants recruited primary and secondary auditory processing regions in response to both the native and novel speech conditions (Fig. 1). Group comparisons showed that HL infants exhibited hypoactivity in response to each language condition, relative to TL infants, in left temporal areas associated with language (Fig. 2; see Table 2 for coordinate table). During the novel language condition only, compared to TL infants the HL group also showed hypoactivity in additional prefrontal (superior frontal and anterior cingulate gyri), parietal (left precuneus and posterior cingulate gyrus), and cerebellar (lobules VIIb and VIIIa, right VIIIb, and left crus I and II) regions, as well as the left hippocampus. Moreover, whereas the HL group did not show differential responses for the two language conditions, the TL group showed significantly greater cerebellar activation in the bilateral crus II, left lobule VIIb, left lobule VIIIa, and left lobule VI during the novel language condition relative to the native condition (Fig. 3). A very similar and largely overlapping cerebellar cluster was also present in the interaction contrast (TL > HL, Japanese > English; Fig. 3). Local activation peaks for the interaction were identified in the left Crus I and II, Lobules VIIb and VIIIb, and in the vermis (Table 2). All group differences held when controlling for potential confounds (sex, maternal education, and birth order) in post-hoc tests (see Table S1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Neural activation in response to each language within each likelihood group: English is shown in red, Japanese is shown in blue, and the overlap is shown in purple. These maps were thresholded at Z > 2.3, and cluster-corrected for multiple comparisons at P < 0.05

Fig. 2
figure 2

Between-group comparisons of neural activation in response to English and Japanese. Results were thresholded at Z > 2.3 and cluster-corrected for multiple comparisons at P < 0.05

Fig. 3
figure 3

Between-language comparisons of neural activation within the TL group (top), and in an interaction between the TL and HL groups (bottom). Results were thresholded at Z > 2.3 and cluster-corrected for multiple comparisons at P < 0.05

ROI analyses

Activation Strength. We found a significant interaction between likelihood group and language condition on activation collapsed across all ROIs and hemispheres (F(1,26)=4.82, p=0.037; Fig. 4A). Post-hoc analyses showed a simple main effect of likelihood group in the novel language condition only (F(1,33)=6.589, p=0.015), such that the TL group had significantly stronger activation for the novel language than the HL group did. We also saw a simple main effect of language condition for the TL group only (F(1,9)=5.292, p=0.047), such that the TL group exhibited stronger activation during the novel language condition than to the native condition across ROIs. Together, these results indicate that the TL infants exhibit not only greater activation in response to language, but also greater neural differentiation between their native language and a foreign language. Additionally, there was a significant interaction between likelihood group and hemisphere (F(1,26)=4.619, p=0.041; Fig. 4B) such that the TL group had significantly greater left-hemisphere activation across all ROIs and language conditions as compared to the HL group (F(1,32)=6.597, p=0.015).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Violin plots showing ANCOVA results in language ROIs. Standard error is shown in error bars. A: Activation Strength for each language, in each likelihood group. B: Activation Strength for each hemisphere, in each likelihood group. C: Mean Laterality Indices of Activation Strength for each language, in each likelihood group. D: Mean Laterality indices of Cluster Size in each likelihood group

Next, when examining laterality indices of activation strength, we found a significant interaction between likelihood group and language condition (F(1,36)=7.921, p=0.008; Fig. 4C). Post-hoc analyses showed that the TL group had stronger left-lateralization during the native language condition, compared to stronger right-lateralization in the HL group, across all ROIs (F(1,40)=6.427, p=0.015).

Cluster Size. When examining cluster size as a variable of interest, we found no significant differences between likelihood groups for either language condition. However, when examining the laterality index based on cluster size, we found a main effect of likelihood group (F(1,34)=7.754, p=0.009) such that the TL group had significantly greater left-lateralization than the HL group across language areas (Fig. 4D). Post-hoc t-tests further showed that the laterality index was significantly left-lateralized in the TL group (t = 2.06, p = 0.027) and right-lateralized in the HL group (t = 1.91, p = 0.033).

Table 2 Coordinate table for activation peaks within likelihood groups and language conditions, in comparisons between likelihood groups, and in comparisons between languages. Peaks Z > 3.1 reported. TL: typical likelihood; HL: high likelihood; L: left; R: right

Discussion

In the present study, we examined neural responses to native and novel speech in order to investigate how the neural signatures of native language processing may differ in a sample of nine-month-old infants at high and typical familial likelihood for autism. As compared to the TL group, HL infants showed diminished neural responses in left temporal language areas for both their native language and a novel language. Furthermore, HL infants also showed no evidence of neural discrimination between the two languages, unlike the TL group, who showed a neural novelty response when presented with the unfamiliar language. In a subsequent a priori analysis that focused on canonical language regions, HL infants also showed patterns of atypically lateralized speech processing, in terms of both strength and spatial extent of neural activation. To our knowledge, this is the first study to use fMRI to directly index autism-associated atypicalities related to native language acquisition in infancy.

Consistent with prior literature showing reliable bilateral activation of temporal language areas in sleeping infants passively exposed to speech [25, 38, 62], here we observed activation in temporal language areas for both groups and for both the native and novel languages. In line with prior work examining speech processing in HL infants [36] and in toddlers with an ASD diagnosis [28, 30], we found that HL infants exhibited a general pattern of hypoactivation across both languages. Thus, we extend prior findings that have linked autism with diminished neural responses to speech in early development. Across both languages, HL infants reliably showed attenuated responses to speech in left temporal language areas. Prior work has shown that similar patterns of temporal hypoactivation predict poorer language skills in autistic toddlers [27, 28]. During exposure to the novel language, HL infants also exhibited hypoactivity in additional brain areas related to attention, memory, and cognitive function, as well as in several cerebellar lobules. The limited responses in these regions suggest that novel social input may not elicit increased attention in HL infants as it does in the TL group. In other words, the greater engagement of these non-canonical language regions (e.g., anterior and posterior cingulate, medial prefrontal cortex, hippocampus) likely reflects increased attention that novel social stimuli naturally elicit in the typically developing brain. Indeed, a “novelty preference” has been well established in the behavioral literature on late infancy [9], and is generally considered a marker of increased attention, social interest, and even active learning. In more recent behavioral language studies of 9- [63] and 10-month-old [64] infants, for example, the presence of a behavioral novelty preference was directly linked to larger vocabulary size.

Interestingly, compared to their TL counterparts, nine-month-old HL infants displayed no evidence of discrimination between the native and novel languages. The ability to differentiate between native and novel languages at the neural level can be viewed as a proxy for native language acquisition progress and, therefore, we interpret this lack of discrimination as a potential marker of diminished uptake of their native language. By contrast, TL controls showed differential patterns of neural activation, characterized by heightened brain responses to the novel language. Therefore, our observation of a novelty response at the neural level – in the TL group only – may be considered a marker of normative language acquisition, and its absence may provide a potential indicator of early delayed or derailed language uptake in the HL group. Notably, this neural discrimination was most evident in the cerebellum, which has increasingly been acknowledged for its important role in language and social cognition [32, 57, 65] in addition to its crucial motor functions. In adults, not only is the cerebellum important for processing prosody and parsing syntax (reviewed in LeBel & D’Mello [32]) – it is also a component of brain networks for reading in adults [66] and children [67], and lesions to the right lateral aspect are known to induce deficits in verbal fluency [66]. Indeed, several regions where TL infants showed this neural novelty response – lobules VI, VIIb, and VIIIa – were previously identified as having a functional role in language according to a study that used a battery of tasks to map the functional topography of the cerebellum [57]. Cerebellar crus I and II also responded more strongly during exposure to the novel language in the present study. This is especially interesting given that these areas of the posterior cerebellum are part of reciprocal circuits that project to prefrontal, temporal, and parietal language areas of the cortex [32]. Crus I in particular is consistently implicated in language functions (see, e.g., [58]). Damage to the right crus I can cause disruptions in phonological fluency and reorganization of left frontal language areas [68]. A recent, more relevant study also observed aberrant cortical functional connectivity with crus I in HL infants with delayed language [34].

The a priori region of interest (ROI) analysis corroborated the results observed in the whole brain analysis. Specifically, the TL group responded significantly more strongly to the novel language than to the native language, across all language ROIs tested, whereas the HL group showed similar activation strength in language ROIs across both languages. The significantly stronger activation to the novel language in the TL group provides further evidence that, at 9 months of age, a neural novelty response when processing unfamiliar linguistic input may be normative, and may indicate a level of attunement conducive to the uptake of novel social stimuli. Furthermore, using the ROI approach the HL group showed significantly weaker left-hemisphere activation across language ROIs during both languages compared to TL infants, directly in line with our earlier findings of weaker left temporal activity observed in the whole-brain analysis. While bilateral temporal language processing is present in typically-developing neonates soon after birth [25], the left hemisphere becomes increasingly specialized for native speech within the first few months of life [62, 69, 70] and this lateralization becomes stronger as language skills develop [71]. Interestingly, one fNIRS study [72] found that native speech processing in newborns already shows some leftward bias in temporal areas, whereas the unfamiliar prosodic patterns of a novel language elicit a rightward bias. Indeed, our findings in this 9-month-old sample showed a lack of leftward lateralization in novel language processing for both groups.

Importantly, our laterality analysis in language ROIs showed that, during the native English condition, specifically, HL infants had strikingly atypical patterns of lateralization with regard to neural activation strength, revealing a failure to develop normative left-lateralization. Additionally, the HL group exhibited larger clusters of activation in the right hemisphere than in the left during speech processing across both languages. Failures to develop left-lateralized speech processing in early life [29], as well as atypically right-lateralized speech processing at older ages, are well documented in autistic individuals from toddlerhood [29, 30] to adolescence [41] and even in adulthood [73]. Therefore, this finding extends previously reported atypicalities to an earlier developmental age. A number of studies have linked diminished leftward language lateralization with poorer language abilities in children and adults with ASD [74, 75]. The existing literature suggests a general pattern in which an early rightward bias in language processing may be detrimental, but some studies in older individuals have found that volumetric [76] and functional [41] rightward lateralization in frontal areas predicted better language abilities in older ASD individuals. Thus, studies in older individuals suggest that some amount of rightward language lateralization later in development may be compensatory in autism [29, 41]. Further longitudinal investigations of language lateralization are needed to characterize the degree to which its autism-associated atypicalities are compensatory, as opposed to detrimental, throughout development.

Although prior studies have used EEG to uncover differences in native versus novel language processing in HL infants [39, 77], the present study is the first to investigate their neural substrates using fMRI. Prior fMRI studies of language processing in HL infants have compared speech against environmental sounds [36] or have investigated statistical word learning using an artificial language [38]. Therefore, the native vs. novel paradigm utilized in the present study addresses an important gap in the literature: by comparing the neural correlates of native language processing against neural responses to another natural language, we are able to tap into the neural substrates subserving language learning, allowing us to more directly index the degree to which language learning has occurred.

Limitations

We acknowledge several limitations of the present study. Foremost, the sample size, particularly in the TL group, was quite modest. Achieving a sample size suitable for analysis was challenging given that passive fMRI auditory paradigms frequently awaken sleeping participants and induce motion artifacts. Recruitment was especially challenging for the TL group: the discrepancy between groups is because the HL sample was largely recruited through UCLA’s Center for Autism Research & Treatment, which is tapped into a local network of families of children with ASD who are enthusiastic about autism research. The TL group was not recruited through any such network of families. We also experienced challenges in participant retention because of the COVID-19 pandemic, which caused sample attrition and the loss of behavioral and diagnostic follow-up data. Moreover, the groups were not matched on some variables that could introduce confounds: the TL group skewed significantly more female and had higher maternal education, and also had a substantial number of firstborn participants. However, our post-hoc analyses indicate that these findings held even when accounting for these potential confounding factors (Table S1). Additionally, the HL group is expected to be more developmentally heterogeneous than the TL group: some HL participants will later develop autism, while others will develop language delays, and still others will have no discernible atypicalities. Thus, our treatment of HL infants as a monolithic group is not optimal, but necessary due to the limited sample size and due to some participants missing diagnostic outcome. Another limitation, shared by virtually all infant neuroimaging studies, is that we were unable to collect data on sleep stage during the MRI scan. Thus, stimulus-evoked paradigms are generally unable to control for the potential confounding effect of sleep stage on neural activity. However, one study in ASD and TD toddlers [78] recently tested the effect of sleep stage (proxied by the amount of time between falling asleep and the scan start) on BOLD activity in primary auditory cortex. BOLD measures did not correlate with time between falling asleep and the start of the MRI, and similarly did not differ between discrete resting-state functional runs, when infants would presumably be in different sleep stages. Furthermore, a sizable body of work in infant participants shows that observing robust neural activation during sleep is not only possible, but well established (e.g., [25, 36, 38]). Future studies using native vs. novel language paradigms should also seek to collect longitudinal fMRI data at several stages of infant development. There is also a conspicuous gap in the native versus novel language processing literature in typical development, which is important for developing a baseline understanding of the neural correlates of native language acquisition. Doing this at the level of large-scale neuroimaging consortia would be especially impactful, as a large longitudinal study would produce a detailed understanding of normative developmental trajectories against which to compare atypical development.

Conclusions

In sum, the present study extends prior work on the neurodevelopmental basis of language delay in autism by investigating how the neural signatures of native language processing differ in 9-month-old infants at high and typical familial likelihood for autism. HL infants showed diminished neural responses to speech in left temporal language areas during both languages, as well as an absence of neural differentiation between native and novel speech. We also extend prior reports of atypical language lateralization in autism to this earlier developmental age. To our knowledge, this is the first study that used fMRI to directly index autism-associated atypicalities related to native language acquisition in infancy. This work furthers our understanding of the neural correlates of language acquisition in autism, which is a critical first step to develop earlier, timelier interventions that could alleviate or mitigate language impairments seen in autistic children and HL siblings.

Data availability

The data used in these analyses are available via the National Institutes of Mental Health Data Archive. Researchers can submit a Data Access Request to access de-identified human subjects data at https://nda.nih.gov/.

Abbreviations

ASD:

Autism Spectrum Disorder

HL:

High Likelihood for Autism

TL:

Typical Likelihood for Autism

fMRI:

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging

MSEL:

Mullen Scales of Early Learning

ELC:

Early Learning Composite

VABS-II:

Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition

MCDI:

MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory

ESCS:

Early Social Communication Scales

RJA:

Responding to Joint Attention

AOSI:

Murashige and Skoog

NAA:

Autism Observation Scale for Infants

ADOS-2:

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule - 2nd Edition

ROI:

Region of Interest

References

  1. Budisteanu M, Linca F, Andrei LE, Mateescu L, Glangher A, Ioana D, et al. Recognition of early warning signs and symptoms - the first steps on the road to Autism Spectrum Disorder diagnosis. Ann Ist Super Sanita. 2022;58(3):183–91.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Mody M, Belliveau JW. Speech and Language impairments in Autism: insights from Behavior and Neuroimaging. N Am J Med Sci (Boston). 2013;5(3):157–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Maenner MJ, Prevalence, and Characteristics of Autism Spectrum Disorder Among Children Aged. 8 Years — AutismDevelopmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 Sites, United States, 2020. MMWR Surveill Summ [Internet]. 2023 [cited 2023 May 9];72. Available from: https://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/volumes/72/ss/ss7202a1.htm

  4. Rose V, Trembath D, Keen D, Paynter J. The proportion of minimally verbal children with autism spectrum disorder in a community-based early intervention programme. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2016;60(5):464–77.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Zwaigenbaum L, Bauman ML, Stone WL, Yirmiya N, Estes A, Hansen RL, et al. Early identification of Autism Spectrum Disorder: recommendations for practice and research. Pediatrics. 2015;136(Supplement1):S10–40.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Kisilevsky BS, Hains SMJ, Brown CA, Lee CT, Cowperthwaite B, Stutzman SS, et al. Fetal sensitivity to properties of maternal speech and language. Infant Behav Dev. 2009;32(1):59–71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Jardri R, Houfflin-Debarge V, Delion P, Pruvo JP, Thomas P, Pins D. Assessing fetal response to maternal speech using a noninvasive functional brain imaging technique. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2012;30(2):159–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Moon C, Cooper RP, Fifer WP. Two-day-olds prefer their native language. Infant Behav Dev. 1993;16(4):495–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Rose SA, Gottfried AW, Melloy-Carminar P, Bridger WH. Familiarity and novelty preferences in infant recognition memory: implications for information processing. Dev Psychol. 1982;18(5):704–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Elsabbagh M, Hohenberger A, Campos R, Van Herwegen J, Serres J, de Schonen S, et al. Narrowing perceptual sensitivity to the native Language in Infancy: exogenous influences on developmental timing. Behav Sci (Basel). 2013;3(1):120–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Werker JF, Tees RC. Cross-language speech perception: evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behav Dev. 1984;7(1):49–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Kasari C, Sigman M, Mundy P, Yirmiya N. Affective sharing in the context of joint attention interactions of normal, autistic, and mentally retarded children. J Autism Dev Disord. 1990;20(1):87–100.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Loveland KA, Landry SH. Joint attention and language in autism and developmental language delay. J Autism Dev Disord. 1986;16(3):335–49.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Miller M, Iosif AM, Hill M, Young GS, Schwichtenberg AJ, Ozonoff S. Response to name in infants developing Autism Spectrum disorder: a prospective study. J Pediatr. 2017;183:141–e1461.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Marschik PB, Widmann CAA, Lang S, Kulvicius T, Boterberg S, Nielsen-Saines K, et al. Emerging verbal functions in early infancy: lessons from Observational and computational approaches on typical Development and Neurodevelopmental disorders. Adv Neurodev Disord. 2022;6(4):369–88.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Maestro S, Muratori F, Cavallaro MC, Pei F, Stern D, Golse B, et al. Attentional skills during the first 6 months of age in autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41(10):1239–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Brisson J, Martel K, Serres J, Sirois S, Adrien JL. Acoustic analysis of oral productions of infants later diagnosed with autism and their mother. Infant Ment Health J. 2014;35(3):285–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Zappella M, Einspieler C, Bartl-Pokorny KD, Krieber M, Coleman M, Bölte S, et al. What do home videos tell us about early motor and socio-communicative behaviours in children with autistic features during the second year of life – an exploratory study. Early Hum Dev. 2015;91(10):569–75.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Patten E, Belardi K, Baranek GT, Watson LR, Labban JD, Oller DK. Vocal patterns in infants with Autism Spectrum Disorder: canonical babbling status and vocalization frequency. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(10):2413–28.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Yankowitz LD, Petrulla V, Plate S, Tunc B, Guthrie W, Meera SS, et al. Infants later diagnosed with autism have lower canonical babbling ratios in the first year of life. Mol Autism. 2022;13:28.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Hyman SL, Levy SE, Myers SM, COUNCIL ON CHILDREN WITH DISABILITIES SODABP, Kuo DZ, Apkon S, et al. Identification, evaluation, and management of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Pediatrics. 2020;145(1):e20193447.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Hall-Lande J, Esler AN, Hewitt A, Gunty AL. Age of initial identification of Autism Spectrum Disorder in a Diverse Urban Sample. J Autism Dev Disord. 2021;51(3):798–803.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Martinez M, Thomas KC, Williams CS, Christian R, Crais E, Pretzel R, et al. Family experiences with the diagnosis of Autism Spectrum disorder: system barriers and facilitators of efficient diagnosis. J Autism Dev Disord. 2018;48(7):2368–78.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Clark MLE, Vinen Z, Barbaro J, Dissanayake C. School Age outcomes of children diagnosed early and later with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2018;48(1):92–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Perani D, Saccuman MC, Scifo P, Anwander A, Awander A, Spada D, et al. Neural language networks at birth. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(38):16056–61.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Friederici AD, Gierhan SM. The language network. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013;23(2):250–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Xiao Y, Wen TH, Kupis L, Eyler LT, Goel D, Vaux K, et al. Neural responses to affective speech, including motherese, map onto clinical and social eye tracking profiles in toddlers with ASD. Nat Hum Behav. 2022;6(3):443–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Lombardo MV, Pierce K, Eyler LT, Carter Barnes C, Ahrens-Barbeau C, Solso S, et al. Different functional neural substrates for Good and Poor Language Outcome in Autism. Neuron. 2015;86(2):567–77.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Eyler LT, Pierce K, Courchesne E. A failure of left temporal cortex to specialize for language is an early emerging and fundamental property of autism. Brain. 2012;135(3):949–60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Redcay E, Courchesne E. Deviant functional magnetic resonance imaging patterns of brain activity to Speech in 2–3-Year-old children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64(7):589–98.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Liu J, Tsang T, Jackson L, Ponting C, Jeste SS, Bookheimer SY, et al. Altered lateralization of dorsal language tracts in 6-week-old infants at risk for autism. Dev Sci. 2019;22(3):e12768.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. LeBel A, D’Mello AM. A seat at the (language) table: incorporating the cerebellum into frameworks for language processing. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2023;53:101310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Van Overwalle F, Manto M, Cattaneo Z, Clausi S, Ferrari C, Gabrieli JDE, et al. Consensus Paper: Cerebellum and Social Cognition. Cerebellum. 2020;19(6):833–68.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Okada NJ, Liu J, Tsang T, Nosco E, McDonald NM, Cummings KK et al. Atypical cerebellar functional connectivity at 9 months of age predicts delayed socio-communicative profiles in infants at high and low risk for autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry [Internet]. 2021 [cited 2022 Apr 2];n/a(n/a). Available from: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/jcpp.13555

  35. Ozonoff S, Young GS, Carter A, Messinger D, Yirmiya N, Zwaigenbaum L, et al. Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: a Baby Siblings Research Consortium study. Pediatrics. 2011;128(3):e488–495.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Blasi A, Lloyd-Fox S, Sethna V, Brammer MJ, Mercure E, Murray L, et al. Atypical processing of voice sounds in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Cortex. 2015;71:122–33.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Pecukonis M, Perdue KL, Wong J, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA. Exploring the relation between brain response to speech at 6-months and language outcomes at 24-months in infants at high and low risk for autism spectrum disorder: a preliminary functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2021;47:100897.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Liu J, Tsang T, Ponting C, Jackson L, Jeste SS, Bookheimer SY, et al. Lack of neural evidence for implicit language learning in 9-month-old infants at high risk for autism. Dev Sci. 2021;24(4):e13078.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Seery AM, Vogel-Farley V, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA. Atypical lateralization of ERP response to native and non-native speech in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2013;5:10–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Morrel J, Singapuri K, Landa RJ, Reetzke R. Neural correlates and predictors of speech and language development in infants at elevated likelihood for autism: a systematic review. Front Hum Neurosci. 2023;17:1211676.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Knaus TA, Burns C, Kamps J, Foundas AL. Atypical activation of action-semantic network in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Brain Cogn. 2017;117:57–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Mullen EM. Mullen scales of early learning. Circle Pines, MN: AGS; 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Sparrow SS, Cicchetti D, Balla DA. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition [Internet]. 2012 [cited 2024 Apr 4]. Available from: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/t15164-000

  44. Fenson L. MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventories. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Mundy P, Delgado C, Block J, Venezia M, Hogan A, Seibert J. Early social communication scales (ESCS). Coral Gables, FL: University of Miami; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Bryson S, Zweigenbaum L, McDermott C, Rombough V, Brian J. The Autism Observation Scale for Infants: Scale Development and Reliability Data| SpringerLink [Internet]. 2008 [cited 2022 Jul 23]. Available from: https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10803-007-0440-y

  47. Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore P, Risi S, Gotham K, Bishop S. Autism diagnostic observation schedule-2nd edition (ADOS-2). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Corporation; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Sundara M, Polka L, Molnar M. Development of coronal stop perception: bilingual infants keep pace with their monolingual peers. Cognition. 2008;108(1):232–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Raschle N, Zuk J, Ortiz-Mantilla S, Sliva DD, Franceschi A, Grant PE, et al. Pediatric neuroimaging in early childhood and infancy: challenges and practical guidelines. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2012;1252(1):43–50.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Spann MN, Wisnowski JL, Smyser CD, Howell B, Dean DC. The art, Science, and secrets of scanning Young Children. Biol Psychiatry. 2023;93(10):858–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Johansen-Berg H, et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL - ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. 2004;23(Supplement 1):S208–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Shi F, Yap PT, Wu G, Jia H, Gilmore JH, Lin W, et al. Infant brain atlases from neonates to 1- and 2-Year-Olds. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(4):e18746.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Liu J, Okada NJ, Cummings KK, Jung J, Patterson G, Bookheimer SY, et al. Emerging atypicalities in functional connectivity of language-related networks in young infants at high familial risk for ASD. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2020;45:100814.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Wagner L, Banchik M, Okada NJ, McDonald N, Jeste SS, Bookheimer SY, et al. Associations between thalamocortical functional connectivity and sensory over-responsivity in infants at high likelihood for ASD. Cereb Cortex. 2023;33(12):8075–86.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Power JD, Mitra A, Laumann TO, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. NeuroImage. 2014;84. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.048.

  56. Eklund A, Nichols TE, Knutsson H. Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2016;113(28):7900–5.

  57. Stoodley CJ, Valera EM, Schmahmann JD. Functional topography of the cerebellum for motor and cognitive tasks: an fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2012;59(2):1560–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. King M, Hernandez-Castillo CR, Poldrack RA, Ivry RB, Diedrichsen J. Functional boundaries in the human cerebellum revealed by a multi-domain task battery. Nat Neurosci. 2019;22(8):1371–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Alexander B, Murray AL, Loh WY, Matthews LG, Adamson C, Beare R, et al. A new neonatal cortical and subcortical brain atlas: the Melbourne Children’s Regional Infant Brain (M-CRIB) atlas. NeuroImage. 2017;147:841–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Adani S, Cepanec M. Sex differences in early communication development: behavioral and neurobiological indicators of more vulnerable communication system development in boys. Croat Med J. 2019;60(2):141–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Hoff-Ginsberg E. The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to children’s language experience and language development. Appl Psycholinguist. 1998;19(4):603–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Dehaene-Lambertz G, Montavont A, Jobert A, Allirol L, Dubois J, Hertz-Pannier L, et al. Language or music, mother or Mozart? Structural and environmental influences on infants’ language networks. Brain Lang. 2010;114(2):53–65.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Marino C, Gervain J. The Novelty Effect as a Predictor of Language Outcome. Frontiers in Psychology [Internet]. 2019 [cited 2023 Oct 25];10. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00258

  64. DePaolis RA, Keren-Portnoy T, Vihman M. Making sense of infant familiarity and novelty responses to words at Lexical Onset. Front Psychol. 2016;7:715.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Mariën P, Borgatti R. Language and the cerebellum. Handb Clin Neurol. 2018;154:181–202.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Stoodley CJ, Stein JF. Cerebellar function in developmental dyslexia. Cerebellum. 2013;12(2):267–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Li H, Kepinska O, Caballero JN, Zekelman L, Marks RA, Uchikoshi Y, et al. Decoding the role of the cerebellum in the early stages of reading acquisition. Cortex. 2021;141:262–79.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Riva D, Taddei M, Ghielmetti F, Erbetta A, Bulgheroni S. Language Cerebro-cerebellar reorganization in children after surgery of right cerebellar astrocytoma: a fMRI study. Cerebellum. 2019;18(4):791–806.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Dehaene-Lambertz G, Dehaene S, Hertz-Pannier L. Functional neuroimaging of Speech Perception in infants. Science. 2002;298(5600):2013–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Shultz S, Vouloumanos A, Bennett RH, Pelphrey K. Neural specialization for speech in the first months of life. Dev Sci. 2014;17(5):766–74.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Dehaene-Lambertz G, Hertz-Pannier L, Dubois J. Nature and nurture in language acquisition: anatomical and functional brain-imaging studies in infants. Trends Neurosci. 2006;29(7):367–73.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Vannasing P, Florea O, González-Frankenberger B, Tremblay J, Paquette N, Safi D, et al. Distinct hemispheric specializations for native and non-native languages in one-day-old newborns identified by fNIRS. Neuropsychologia. 2016;84:63–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Anderson JS, Nielsen JA, Froehlich AL, DuBray MB, Druzgal TJ, Cariello AN, et al. Functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging classification of autism. Brain. 2011;134(12):3742–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Cermak CA, Arshinoff S, Ribeiro de Oliveira L, Tendera A, Beal DS, Brian J, et al. Brain and Language associations in Autism Spectrum disorder: a scoping review. J Autism Dev Disord. 2022;52(2):725–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Lindell AK, Hudry K. Atypicalities in cortical structure, handedness, and functional lateralization for Language in Autism Spectrum disorders. Neuropsychol Rev. 2013;23(3):257–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Joseph RM, Fricker Z, Fenoglio A, Lindgren KA, Knaus TA, Tager-Flusberg H. Structural asymmetries of language-related gray and white matter and their relationship to language function in young children with ASD. Brain Imaging Behav. 2014;8(1):60–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Finch KH, Seery AM, Talbott MR, Nelson CA, Tager-Flusberg H. Lateralization of ERPs to speech and handedness in the early development of Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Neurodev Disord. 2017;9:4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Linke AC, Chen B, Olson L, Ibarra C, Fong C, Reynolds S, et al. Sleep problems in Preschoolers with Autism Spectrum Disorder are Associated with sensory sensitivities and thalamocortical overconnectivity. Biol Psychiatry: Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2023;8(1):21–31.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful for the generous support of the Brain Mapping Medical Research Organization, Brain Mapping Support Foundation, Pierson-Lovelace Foundation, the Ahmanson Foundation, Capital Group Companies Charitable Foundation, William M. and Linda R. Dietel Philanthropic Fund, and Northstar Fund. The content contained herein is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. We sincerely thank the families who have generously donated their time to make this research possible.

Funding

This work was supported by the Eunice Kennedi Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development at the National Institutes of Health grant number P50 HD055784 to S.Y.B., S.S.J., and M.D.; the National Institute of Mental Health at the National Institutes of Health grant number R01 MH100028 to M.D.; and the National Institutes of Health grant number T32 HD091059 and F31 HD108987 to L.W.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

LW: methodology, data processing, data analysis, visualization, writing – original draft, writing – review and editing, revision and finalization; MB: data acquisition, data processing, data curation, writing – review and editing; TT: methodology, writing – review and editing; NJO: data acquisition, data processing, data analysis, writing – review and editing; RA: visualization, data processing; NM: writing – review and editing; SYB: funding acquisition, conceptualization; SSJ: funding acquisition, conceptualization, writing – review and editing; SAG: supervision, writing – review and editing; MD: funding acquisition, conceptualization, supervision, writing – review and editing.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mirella Dapretto.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Informed consent was obtained from all participants’ caregivers and/or legal guardians. All study protocols were approved by the UCLA Institutional Review Board.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wagner, L., Banchik, M., Tsang, T. et al. Atypical early neural responses to native and non-native language in infants at high likelihood for developing autism. Molecular Autism 16, 6 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13229-025-00640-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13229-025-00640-w

Keywords