- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Altered interactive dynamics of gaze behavior during face-to-face interaction in autistic individuals: a dual eye-tracking study
Molecular Autism volume 16, Article number: 12 (2025)
Abstract
Background
Previous eye-tracking research on autistic individuals has mostly examined the gaze behavior of one individual in response to social stimuli presented on a computer screen, suggesting that there is atypical gaze behavior. However, it is unknown how these findings translate to the interactive dynamics of gaze behavior during “face-to-face” encounters between two individuals. Only by analyzing the gaze behaviour of both interaction partners is it possible to determine the frequency of actual eye-contact and who initiates or breaks such periods of mutual eye gaze. The knowledge gained from this analysis could contribute to theorizing about the psychological mechanisms (e.g., gaze avoidance vs. gaze indifference) underlying autism.
Methods
The present study applied a novel dual eye-tracking setup that allows the assessment and analysis of the interactive dynamics of gaze behavior regarding (i) mutual eye gaze (i.e., eye contact), (ii) initiations, and (iii) break-ups of eye contact. Participants (37 autistic individuals, 37 age- and IQ-matched neurotypical individuals) performed a semi-standardized social interaction (i.e., Fast Friends Procedure) with a confederate (trained to interact in a standardized manner).
Results
Eye contact was reduced in interactions involving autistic individuals. Additional analyses revealed that this reduction was primarily due to the more frequent breaking of eye contact by these individuals. We also found considerable heterogeneity among autistic individuals, with atypical gaze behavior present in only about half of the sample.
Limitations
Further research is required to determine whether the interactive dynamics of gaze behavior observed in this dual eye-tracking setup can be generalized to real-world situations. Future studies could also include arousal-related physiological measures.
Conclusions
By tracking the gaze behavior of two interacting individuals, this study reveals specific atypicalities in the interactive dynamics of gaze behavior in a subset of autistic individuals, potentially informing diagnostic and therapeutic decisions. More broadly, our study highlights the added value of dual eye-tracking in elucidating the interactive nature of social encounters in both neurodiverse and neurotypical individuals.
Trial registration
The study was registered as a clinical trial before starting data collection (https://drks.de/search/en/trial/DRKS00018957; Registration Date: 12/17/2019).
Background
Imagine a scenario where two individuals engage in a conversation, discussing their interests and sharing personal stories. As they exchange words, their eyes both receive and send pivotal social information (e.g., turn-taking, listening, signalling social interest, [1, 2]), enabling them to continually monitor and dynamically adapt their behavior depending on their interaction partner’s behavior [3, 4]. This enables humans to establish smooth and reciprocal communication during social encounters. However, for autistic individuals, this fundamental aspect of human interaction often presents challenges, raising questions about how their atypical gaze behavior [5, 6] impacts the dynamics of face-to-face exchanges. Although research on gaze behavior in autism has yielded less consistent findings than we might expect from clinical observations, at least in adult populations there is evidence that autistic individuals tend to exhibit briefer fixation duration towards the eyes and face in general and longer fixation duration towards non-social regions [7,8,9,10]. However, the vast majority of these studies were conducted in highly standardized laboratory settings involving the passive observation of social stimuli (e.g., faces, videos) presented on a computer screen, with no actual social interaction taking place. A few pioneering studies have investigated gaze behavior of autistic individuals in natural social interactions [11,12,13,14], but none of these studies applied dual eye-tracking to illuminate the interactive nature of gaze behavior in two individuals. It is therefore still unclear whether the atypicalities observed in “face-to-screen” experiments generalize to more ecologically valid “face-to-face” social interactions. Most importantly, the question of which aspects of gaze behavior’s interactive dynamics (e.g., initiations of eye contact, break-ups of eye contact) might be altered in autistic individuals remains unanswered. Capitalizing on a novel dual eye-tracking setup with no devices interfering with natural eye contact (see Fig. 1), we aimed to compare these interactive dynamics across autistic individuals and an age- and IQ-matched sample of neurotypical individuals.
Note. The experimental dual eye-tracking setup in which both individuals face each other with the remote eye-tracking devices placed in the middle of the table and two cameras above each head recording the participants’ field of view (visualisation after [15]). a Example frame of the left individual’s gaze behavior on the facial features of the individual sitting opposite b and example frame for the OpenFace analysis of the right individual’s facial features c. We obtained informed consent from all displayed individuals for publishing identifiable images
It is essential to investigate interactive gaze behavior while two individuals are interacting socially [3, 4, 8]. First, another individual’s actual physical presence creates a social setting fundamentally different from passively viewing social stimuli while sitting alone in front of a computer screen. Actually meeting the other individual might trigger somewhat higher emotional arousal due to the awareness of how the other individual might evaluate one’s own behavior and how one should behave in such a social situation. Second, interacting individuals communicate reciprocally with each other via their gaze behavior. Tracking the gaze behavior of two interacting individuals provides new opportunities to analyze the interactive dynamics of gaze behavior characteristics, opportunities that are not available in studies tracking only a single individual’s gaze behavior. Even if one assesses the eye gaze of one individual who is interacting with another individual [11,12,13,14], we gain no knowledge about the interaction partner’s gaze behavior, meaning that the interactive dynamics of gaze behavior (e.g., who initiates or breaks eye contact) remain a “black box”. In contrast, dual eye-tracking reveals unique information on how long two individuals hold eye contact (i.e., display mutual eye gaze), and on which of the two interacting individuals is initiating or breaking-up eye contact [15]. Revealing which of these interactive dynamics might be altered in autistic individuals could inform our understanding of the psychological mechanisms underlying autism.
Theoretical accounts propose two main psychological mechanisms underlying atypical gaze behavior associated with autism. On the one hand, the “gaze avoidance account” suggests that autistic individuals actively avoid eye regions to minimize hyperarousal caused by the experienced threat evoked by another individual’s direct gaze [16,17,18,19]. On the other hand, the “gaze indifference account” explains atypical gaze behavior via an diminished propensity to direct attention to salient social stimuli like the eyes and faces [20,21,22]. While both models would predict reduced eye contact in interactions involving autistic individuals, they might also be associated with specific atypicalities of interactive gaze behavior: if autistic individuals experience eye contact as aversive, they should be especially apt to break up eye contact more often. This is because it seems difficult to avoid eye contact from the start in anticipating the aversive arousal, given that attention seems to be spontaneously biased towards faces and eyes [23, 24]. On the other hand, if autistic individuals are indifferent to eye gaze, they in particular should tend to initiate eye contact less often, because the eyes are not perceived as engaging. Once attention has been directed towards the eyes and eye contact has been established, we do not assume that gaze indifference would result in higher probability to break up eye contact [20]. In sum, analyzing interactive gaze behavior has the potential to shed some light on the psychological mechanisms of atypical gaze behavior in autistic individuals during naturalistic face-to-face interactions.
The present study illuminates the interactive dynamics of gaze behavior in autistic individuals during a naturalistic social encounter from “face-to-face”. For that purpose, we applied a dual eye-tracking setup that enables the assessment of gaze behavior from two individuals at the same time, while ensuring eye-tracking accuracy within an experimentally controlled, laboratory setting [15, 25]. We compared gaze behavior during a semi-standardized social interaction across autistic individuals and age-, and IQ-matched neurotypical individuals. This interaction involved the Fast Friends Procedure, which simulates a social encounter among strangers through escalating, mutual self-disclosure in a series of turn-taking questions [15, 26]. This procedure also enables us to check whether gaze behavior differs between talking and listening situations. As eye gaze is known to be more intense when listening than when talking [2, 15], we hypothesize that potential atypicalities in interactive gaze behavior are especially pronounced in this social role. To reduce the complexity of this experimental design, all participants interacted with a same-sex confederate blind to their interaction partner’s group and trained to interact and provide eye contact in a standardized manner. We analyzed both one-directional (e.g., unilateral gaze; see Methods for details) and interactive gaze parameters (e.g., eye contact, initiations, and break-ups of eye contact; see Methods for details) and also tested for the effect of “listening” vs. “speaking”. Controlling for the confederate’s gaze behavior, we thereby aimed to provide insights into which specific atypicalities of the interactive dynamics of gaze behavior (e.g., fewer initiations of eye contact, more break-ups of eye contact) may underlie the hypothetical reduced eye contact during naturalistic, social interactions in autistic individuals. We also aimed to illuminate sources of heterogeneity within autism [18, 27].
Methods
Participants
The study was approved by the local Ethics Committee (Approval ID: 439/15) and registered as a clinical trial before starting data collection (https://drks.de/search/en/trial/DRKS00018957). Please note that this study was originally planned to be part of a dual-center project aimed at assessing interactive gaze behavior in autistic individuals and social phobia. Due to differences in hygiene and safety regulations during the Covid-19 pandemic in the two involved universities located in two different federal states, data at the two locations were collected independently from each other. As there are higher prevalence of autism in males [28] and potential phenotypical differences across male and female autistic individuals [29, 30] which could not be addressed within this study’s framework, this study focused on male participants (see also Limitations). Thirty-nine adult male autistic individuals (according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) [5], and confirmed by several clinical instruments, see Psychometric Measures and Clinical Instruments) and 39 neurotypical male individuals case-matched for age and total IQ (see Table 1) took part in the experiment. On the basis of a recent meta-analysis suggesting an effect of atypical gaze behavior with g = 0.47–0.50 (Hedges g) for socially complex interactions [9], we conducted an a priori power analyses (using G*Power, version 3.1.9.4, [31]). With a significance criterion of α = 0.05 and power = 0.80, the minimum total sample size needed with this effect size is N = 65. We investigated 78 participants (39 per group) to account for potential drop-outs during the study procedure. Participants were recruited at the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy of the University Medical Center Freiburg, via flyers, and announcements on an institutional notice board and in the local newspaper. To control for any mating effects [32, 33], we included only heterosexually-oriented participants. All participants underwent telephone screening to monitor whether they fulfilled our inclusion criteria: taking no medication, normal to low corrected-to-normal vision (≤ 3 diopters), no other psychiatric condition, no current psychotherapy (for details, see Table S1). Based on data from a simulation study on the applied dual eye-tracking setup [25], we defined cut-offs for sufficient data quality (low quality: accuracy > 1.0° and precision > 0.5°). These cut-offs resulted in the exclusion of two autistic individuals (and the two matched neurotypical individuals). Note that data quality did not differ across groups (all Fs (1, 74) ≤ . 1.44, p ≥ 0.234, η2 ≤ . 0.009, for details see Table S2b) and that including the outliers in the analysis led to similar findings. Our final sample consisted of n = 37 autistic individuals and n = 37 neurotypical individuals (for descriptive statistics and psychometric measures see Table 1). All participants gave written informed consent before the experiment and received €75.00 for their participation.
Setup
The experiment took place in a bright room with constant artificial lighting, a white table (size: 80 cm × 80 cm; height: 72 cm) and two identical and height-adjustable chairs. Chairs could be replaced by a movable vertical calibration wall (180 × 80 cm, nine black crosshairs with a total radius of 3.5 cm and 1.5° visual angle on a white background resulting in a calibration area of 40 × 40 cm and 17.3° × 17.3° visual angle). The dual eye-tracking setup consisted of two Tobii X3 remote infrared eye-trackers (sampling frequency: 120 Hz) and two cameras (Logitech C920; FHD resolution: 1920 × 1080 px with 30fps) arranged to track the gaze behavior of two participants sitting across from each other at a table (for detailed information, see [15, 25]). The two eye trackers were placed on the table and the two cameras were mounted above the participants’ heads. Compared to Tönsing et al. [15], there were only minor changes in distances and angles (angle of remote eye-tracking devices: 28°; height of both scene cameras: 114 cm). Due to the restrictions related to the Covid-19 pandemic, it was necessary to increase the distance between the participant and confederate from 131 to 151 cm for half of the experiment (eye-tracking data quality, one-directional and interactive gaze behavior were not affected, for more details see Tables S2, S3 and S4). A movable green push-button used to synchronize the two eye-tracking data streams and segment the interaction was placed in the middle of the table. See Fig. 1 for further details and sample images and Videos S1-2 for illustration.
The Fast Friends Procedure
The Fast-Friends-Procedure (FFP) scaffolds semi-standardized communication and builds rapport between unknown others through mutual self-disclosure in a series of turn-taking questions [26]. In line with Tönsing et al. [15], we used 12 questions from the original FFP translated into German (e.g., "What would constitute a perfect day for you?"; see Table S5 for details.).
Psychometric measures and clinical instruments
To screen for a wide range of psychopathological characteristics, the Mini-Symptom Check List (Mini-SCL, [34, 35]) was completed by participants. Depressive symptoms were assessed using the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II, [36, 37]). Autistic characteristics were assessed via the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ, [38, 39]). Empathy was assessed using the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI, [40, 41]). Social anxiety was quantified with the Social Interaction Anxiety Scale (SIAS-D, [42, 43]). Subjectively experienced fear and avoidance of eye contact were assessed with the Gaze Anxiety Rating Scale (GARS, [44, 45]). Fearful or worrying cognitions were measured using the Brief Fear of Negative Evaluation (BFNE, [46, 47]). Furthermore, we administered the structured observation instrument Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule 2 module 4 (ADOS-2, [48, 49]) and Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 Disorders-Clinician Version (SCID-5-CV, [50, 51]). We also assessed the revised Culture Fair Test (CFT-20-R, [52, 53]). To measure verbal intelligence, we included the Multiple Choice Vocabulary Test (Wortschatztest, WST, [54]).
Subjective ratings of interaction quality
Participants responded to six items on a visual analog scale (VAS, 1 = not at all, 100 = very much) before and after the interaction. These items assessed shame, anxiety, satisfaction, desire to escape, desire for support, and stress. After the interaction, we presented further items on attractiveness, authenticity, liking, sympathy, own enjoyment, distraction, desire to continue, laughter, role enjoyment, perceived self-disclosure of the other, one’s own openness, and the interaction partner’s openness on the same VAS (for full items see Table S6). We also used the Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale (IOS, [55]) to assess perceived interpersonal closeness (pair of circles to choose from; 1 = no overlap to 7 = most overlap).
Confederates
To ensure comparable interactions for all participants, they interacted with one of six same-sex confederates (age: M = 27.33, SD = 4.03) who were blind to their interaction partner’s group and who were trained to interact in a standardized manner. All confederates should demonstrate an average amount of gaze and facial expression behavior. Furthermore, we took care that the number of experimental sessions with autistic individuals and with neurotypical individuals was counterbalanced for each confederate. They should answer the questions in approximately 30 s (120 s for the last question). All answers should be of the same valence, structure and length; only personal details were adapted for the confederates. Confederates were further trained to adhere to the experimental procedure as closely as possible (e.g., by compensating for too short and too long answers, reacting confidently to unforeseen events and staying on schedule; see Experimental Procedure for more details). We, authors of this manuscript, performed the training and provided detailed video feedback until all requirements for the confederates’ behavior were fulfilled (two confederates had to be excluded because they were unable to behave consistently as trained).
Experimental procedure
Interested participants first completed a short telephone screening (10 min) to check the general inclusion criteria (for more details see Table S1). They were then invited to attend the diagnostic session conducted by a trained and experienced psychologist. Participants first gave informed consent to take part in this study, and to record the diagnostic session and experiment for study purposes. The interviewer administered the CFT-20, SCID-5, and ADOS to all participants. This was followed by the aforementioned psychometric measures, which were completed on a tablet computer. Participants could take a break in between. The entire diagnostic session lasted approximately 2.5 h. Autistic individuals were included in the study if they met the DSM-5 criteria for autism [5] and did not meet the DSM-5 criteria for another psychiatric condition. Of 57 autistic individuals invited to the first appointment, 16 were excluded for failing to meet our criteria (two further autistic individuals did not appear to the second appointment). Neurotypical individuals had to be free of any psychiatric condition (for more details see Table S1).
At the experimental session, participants were first instructed to take part in a social interaction task with a second person (confederate), during which they had to ask and answer prepared questions (FFP). If the participant and confederate were already acquainted, a new appointment was arranged with a different confederate. First, the experimenter brought the confederate (supposedly chosen at random) into the experimental room where the setup (e.g., seat height, chair position) was optimized for the eye-tracking recording. The eye-tracking calibration and validation procedure was then completed (see [15] for details). The participant was waiting in another room during this procedure. After successful calibration, the same procedure was repeated with the participant. When both were seated again, we conducted a final validation procedure in which both participants looked at their opponent’s distinctive facial features (e.g., left eye, right eye, nose, mouth). The experimenter then read a standardized introduction to the social interaction task (FFP) and then left the room. This procedure was first carried out with a practice trial. The confederate read the first question printed on a card, and the participant answered the question first. He had approximately 30 s to answer and was instructed to press a green button before starting to speak. The confederate then pressed the button, answering the same question within 30 s. After 60 s, a beep signaled to both to move on to the next question. The interaction partners had to answer 11 questions with the same pattern of rotating pose-and-answer and time limit (due to its more open framing, the last question had a 120 s time frame for the participant and confederate). The interviews lasted 15 min, and when the dyad had finished the last question, they rang a bell and the experimenter returned to the room. A further facial feature validation procedure was carried out for both participants. For the final part of the experiment, the participant and confederate completed subjective rating items on two tablet computers. After a short break, the participants also performed tasks including screen-based eye-tracking, which we will analyze elsewhere. They were then compensated for their expenses and seen off. In total, the laboratory experiment lasted approximately 2.25 h.
Data analysis
Preprocessing
All analyses were based on averaged binocular data. For eye-tracking validation, segments of eye-tracking data were checked for saccades. In cases of a saccade (e.g., the marker was set before the participant fixated on the cued point), the markers were adjusted (for more details see [25]).
Accuracy, precision, and robustness
Accuracy is defined as the mean offset of the gaze point position relative to the target point position and is reported in degrees of gaze angle. Precision is calculated as a measure of the variance (SD) of data samples in degrees. Robustness includes the percentage of valid eye-tracking data points for a given time sequence. Analyses of the eye-tracking data quality indices were performed using in-house scripts in Matlab (R2018a, version 9.7.0). As can be seen in Table 2, the eye-tracking data quality indices were on a very high level and comparable to previous studies with this setup [15, 25].
Definition of areas of interest
In line with Tönsing et al. [15], we assessed the facial landmark detection tool OpenFace [56] by analyzing the scene videos to generate facial landmarks. The limited-radius Voronoi tessellation method (radius of 2° for 131 cm distance and 1.73° for 151 cm; [57]) was applied to obtain the areas of interest (AOI) eyes (right and left eyes added together), nose, mouth and the rest of the face [25, 58]. An ellipse around the face provided the AOI face.
Analysis of one-directional eye-tracking data
Total dwell-time on a given AOI was defined as the percentage of total frames on the given AOI (e.g., eyes or nose) within a given segment including all gaze behaviors (fixations, saccades, etc.). The percentage of missing data was also calculated including all frames with no data due to gaze outside the calibration area or loss of eye-tracking. For the one-directional gaze analysis, we calculated the average time spent looking at the other person’s eyes as a percentage of total time. We did so for all trials separately, while differentiating between speaking and listening phases, as well as aggregating over both.
Analysis of interactive eye-tracking data
For the analysis of interactive eye-tracking data, we first had to synchronize the two gaze data streams in time and we then classified specific events in that data. Initiations were defined as events where initially both individuals are not looking at each other, and then one individual starts to gaze at the opponent’s eyes for at least 200 ms. The threshold for initiating eye contact was set at 200 ms [15], as this represents the human reaction time to visual stimuli [59, 60]. This threshold was chosen to minimize the number of very short and any random events potentially introducing noise to subsequent analyses. However, different thresholds (0 ms to 1000 ms) revealed similar result patterns (see Table S7). Mutual eye gaze was defined as events where both participants gaze at the opponent’s eyes (we also analyzed mutual face gaze, i.e. when both participants gaze at the opponent’s face, for details see Tables S14 and 15). Finally, we identified break-ups, as events where one individual terminated eye contact. The number of initiations/break-ups was compared to the number of initiations/break-ups by the counterpart, resulting in an initiation/break-up ratio given in percentages. The total duration of mutual eye gaze was related to the duration of the total conversation as well, resulting in mutual eye gaze given as a percentage of total time.
Statistical analysis
Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS (28th edition), RStudio (RStudio version 1.4.1106, R version 4.2.1), and MATLAB (version 2018b) software. The analyses of Bayesian statistics were carried out in JASP (version 0.16.3, 202,161). All tests were performed as two-tailed tests with a significance level of alpha = 0.05. In cases of multiple testing or post hoc testing, Bonferroni correction was applied. In the case of heterogeneity of covariance (Mauchly sphericity test), Greenhouse–Geisser corrections were applied. The Shapiro–Wilk test indicated non-normal distribution of the one-directional and interactive gaze data variables (all Ws ≥ 0.83, ps ≤ 0.001).
For the one-directional and interactive eye-tracking data, we calculated the intraclass correlation (ICC), as a considerable amount of variance could be explained by clusters of dependent data structure (mutual eye gaze: ICC = 0.67). In fact, groups were not independent as they were interacting with the same confederates. Therefore, and as one-directional and interactive eye-tracking data were not normally distributed, we conducted generalized linear mixed models for these variables (GLM, package lme4 in R; [61]). First, to identify the random slope structure with the best fit, models with the full fixed-effects structure were tested. The full fixed-effects models with different random slopes were compared using the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC; [62, 63]). With the optimized random-effects structure, fixed predictors were added to the model step by step, compared, keeping only those effects that significantly improved the predictor model. We applied a z-standardization to all metric variables to provide interpretable model coefficients for GLM. For dependent variables, a generalized linear mixed model with questions and phase (Level 1), nested in group, confederates, distance, and participants (Level 2) was conducted. We tested for effects of group (autistic individuals vs. neurotypical individuals), questions (1 to 11), and phase (speaking or listening). We also checked whether psychometric measures improved the model fit as fixed effects. As random effects, we defined subjects, confederates, and the distance between individuals. We included by-participant slopes for all fixed effects and random intercepts for all possible predictors, adhering to standard guidelines for linear mixed-effects modelling [64].
For comparisons of the autism subgroups (between-subjects factor group) we ran MANOVAs [65]. Bayesian analyses were performed using the Bayesian t-test approach, as non-significant results applying frequentist statistics do not mean that the alternative hypothesis is true [66, 67]. The null hypothesis states that the two groups do not differ in gaze behavior H0: δ = 0. The two-tailed alternative hypothesis (H1) states that the two groups differ from each other. The default prior option provided by JASP was used to assess the likelihood of the alternative hypothesis given the data (BF10) compared to the null hypothesis. The Bayes factor may indicate higher likelihood for one of the hypotheses.
Results
Confederate behavior and ratings
In the first step, we checked whether confederates indeed were able to exhibit standardized gaze behavior when interacting with autistic individuals and neurotypical individuals (factor group). Regarding total dwell-time, there was no significant effect of group for any AOI (all F(1, 74) ≤ 5.48, p ≥ 0.110, η2 ≤ 0.071). The same pattern appeared for total fixation time, mean fixation duration, and total number of fixations (all F(1, 74) ≤ 1.92, p ≥ 0.092, η2 ≤ 0.039; for more details see Table S8).
We also analyzed the confederates’ ratings regarding interaction quality. There was no effect of group for items rated pre and post (all F(1, 74) ≤ 1.15, p ≥ 0.286, η2 ≤ 0.016). Confederates did not differ in rated and desired interpersonal closeness in regard to interacting with autistic individuals or with neurotypical individuals (all F(1, 74) ≤ 2.17, p ≥ 0.145, η2 ≤ 0.028; for more details see Table S9).
One-directional gaze behavior
We first tested whether autistic individuals (compared to neurotypical individuals) gazed less towards facial features and sought any other differences in one-directional gaze behavior (fixations, number of fixations, mean fixation time). For the AOI total dwell-time on the eyes, the generalized linear mixed model with the best fit for the dependent variable total dwell-time on the AOI face contained random slopes for trial (Δ AIC: 191.42) and phase (Δ AIC: 6.65). Additionally, the model contained a fixed effect of group (b = 0.68, SD = 0.18, t(74) = 251.24, p < 0.001; Δ AIC: 231.51) with lower dwell-time levels on the face in autistic individuals (see Fig. 2a). Furthermore, the best model contained a fixed effect of phase (b = 0.04, SD = 0.09, t(74) = 21.27, p < 0.001; Δ AIC: 6.26) with higher dwell-time levels on the face during listening phases (for details see Tables S10 and 11). The interaction effect of group*phase was significant (t(74) = 23.66, p < 0.001) with more pronounced differences in dwell-time between speaking and listening phases in the neurotypical group (see Tables S10 and 11 for more details). There was no effect of time since the variable time did not improve the model. Likewise, the variable confederate’s gaze behavior did not become significant. Adding further variables (e.g., BDI, SIAS, IRI, BFNE) did not significantly improve the model fit (all bs ≤ 0.06, SDs ≤ 0.05, ts(74) ≤ 0.92, ps ≥ 0.361, Δ AICs ≤ 1.74). Models for AOIs nose, mouth, background and rest of face indicated similar effects (see Tables S10 and 11 for more details). We observed similar effects for the variables fixation time (see Table S12) and number of fixations (see Table S13) with shorter mean durations (b ≥ 0.54, SD ≥ 0.18, t(74) ≥ 10.05, p ≤ 0.001; Δ AIC: 12.05; see Tables S14 for more details) and fewer fixations (b ≥ 0.73, SD ≥ 0.22, t(74) ≥ 7.30, p ≤ 0.001; Δ AIC: 2.83; see Tables S14 for more details) in autistic individuals.
Gaze behavior parameter (one-directional and interactive) time for autistic individuals and neurotypical individuals. Note. a Total dwell-time on the eyes during the interaction as a percentage of total time, b mutual eye gaze during the interaction as a percentage of total time, c initiations for mutual eye gaze as a percentage of the total number of initiations, and d break-ups of mutual eye gaze as a percentage of the total number of break-ups by the participant and his interaction partner. Group mean is displayed with bars, standard errors, and individual means (black dot) for each participant
In summary, the analysis of one-directional eye-tracking data indicated differences in groups for several gaze parameters. Autistic individuals gazed less (dwell-time, fixation time, number of fixations) and shorter (fixation time) at facial features as eyes, mouth, and the total face.
Interactive dynamics of gaze behavior
Eye contact
In the next step, we checked whether autistic individuals (compared to neurotypical individuals) engage in less eye contact with the confederate. The generalized linear mixed model with the best fit for the variable mutual eye gaze contained random slopes for phase (Δ AIC: 471.32). The best-fitting model contained a fixed effect of group (b = 0.31, SD = 0.05, t(74) = 56.16, p < 0.001; Δ AIC: 95.17) with lower levels of total mutual eye gaze for autistic individuals (see Fig. 2b). Furthermore, there was a fixed effect of phase (b = 0.36, SD = 0.08, t(74) = 13.11, p < 0.001; Δ AIC: 54.35) with higher levels of mutual eye gaze during listening phases. We detected no effect of time, indicating that mutual eye gaze did not change during the conversation (b = 0.05, SD = 0.06, t(74) = 0.35, p = 0.634). The model also contained a significant interaction effect group*phase (b = 0.26, SD = 0.06, t(74) = 9.15, p < 0.001; Δ AIC: 9.31). As in our unilateral analysis, the difference in mutual eye gaze between speaking and listening phases was less pronounced in autistic individuals than in neurotypical individuals (t(74) = 5.31, p < 0.001). Further variables did not significantly improve the model fit and were not included (e.g., BDI, SIAS, IRI, BFNE; all bs ≤ 0.28, SDs ≤ 0.11, ts(74) ≤ 0.80, ps ≥ 0.423, Δ AICs ≤ 0.83). We noted a similar result pattern in the total mutual face-gaze-duration variable (see Tables S15 and 16 for more details). In sum, we observed less eye contact during interactions involving autistic individuals.
Initiations and break-ups of eye contact
We tested whether autistic individuals (compared to neurotypical individuals) showed atypicalities in additional variables quantifying the interactive dynamics of gaze behavior (i.e., initiations and break-ups of eye contact). The generalized linear mixed model with the best fit for the dependent variable initiations contained a fixed effect of group (b = 0.15, SD = 0.11, t(74) = 54.24, p < 0.001; Δ AIC: 490.03) with lower initiation levels in autistic individuals (see Fig. 2c). Furthermore, the best-fitting model contained a fixed effect of phase (b = 0.40, SD = 0.08, t(74) = 14.05, p < 0.001; Δ AIC: 85.93) with higher levels of initiations during listening phases. The generalized linear mixed model with the best fit for the dependent variable break-ups contained a fixed effect of group (b = 0.25, SD = 0.13, t(74) = 34.74, p < 0.001; Δ AIC: 90.43) with higher levels of break-ups in autistic individuals (see Fig. 2d). Furthermore, the best-fitting model contained a fixed effect of phase (b = 0.34, SD = 0.08, t(74) = 12.47, p < 0.001; Δ AIC: 61.55) with higher levels of break-ups during listening phases. This model also contained a significant interaction effect group*phase (b = 0.39, SD = 0.08, t(74) = 7.21, p < 0.001; Δ AIC: 19.75). Autistic individuals interrupted eye contact particularly often when they were in the response role (see Tables S15 and S16 for more details).
In the next step, we checked whether the reduced eye contact in break-ups involving autistic individuals was driven by less initiations and/or more break-ups of eye contact. We conducted commonality analysis which decomposes the model’s R2 statistic (i.e., explained variance) for mutual eye gaze into commonality coefficients R2 for each participant and confederate event class. Break-ups of mutual gaze events by autistic individuals explained the most variance, R2 = 0.158. As the second variable, fewer initiations accounted for the second most variance, R2 = 0.076. Thus, the reduced mutual eye gaze was largely explained by the more frequent break-ups and, to a lesser degree, by fewer initiations of eye contact by autistic individuals.
In summary, the analysis of interactive eye gaze behavior demonstrated that autistic individuals have less eye contact with the confederate than neurotypical individuals. Additional analyses revealed that reduced eye contact was largely driven by more break-ups by autistic individuals.
Subjective ratings of interaction quality
We also checked whether autistic individuals and neurotypical individuals differed in subjective ratings of interaction quality. For the six items participants rated pre and post, we calculated an ANOVA with repeated measurements. Before the interaction took place, the autism group delivered significant higher scores for “shame”, “fear”, “wish to leave” and “perceived stress”, and lower rates for “happiness” (all F(1, 74) ≥ 4.73, p ≤ 0.033, η2 ≥ 0.059; for more details see Table S16).
After the interaction took place, we noted significant effects with higher levels in the autism group for the items “fear”, “wish to leave” and “stress” and lower levels for the item “happiness” (all F(1, 74) ≥ 4.50, p ≤ 0.037, η2 ≥ 0.059). Furthermore, there were significant effects for time, with "perceived stress” showing lower scores after the interaction, and "fear" and “happiness” showing higher scores (all F(1, 74) ≥ 5.82, p ≤ 0.017, η2 ≥ 0.037). However, we detected no interaction effect of group and time for any of these items (all Fs(1, 74) ≤ 0.48, p ≥ 0.488, η2 ≤ 0.003; see Table S17 for more details). For further post items rated only once after the interaction, the autism group rated the items “enjoyment”, “enjoyment of their role”, and “perceived fun” significantly lower (all F(1, 74) ≥ 5.56, p ≤ 0.021, η2 ≥ 0.069, all other F(1, 74) ≤ 3.15, p ≥ 0.080, η2 ≤ 0.040; for more details see Table S17).
Finally, we analysed associations of the subjective ratings of interaction quality with actual gaze behavior. We found significant associations only for ratings after the interaction took place (pre-interaction ratings: all rs ≤ -0.249, all ps ≥ 0.138): Individuals who experienced more “stress”, “shame”, and “fear” displayed lower levels of mutual eye gaze (all rs ≥ -0.356, all ps ≤ 0.030; see Table S18 for more details); and individuals who experienced more “sympathy” and “fun” (all rs ≥ 0.434, all ps ≤ 0.007; see Table S18 for more details) displayed higher levels of mutual eye gaze. In summary, autistic individuals rated more negative as well as less positive emotions both before and after the social interaction took place, and post-interaction ratings correlated significantly with having eye contact.
Subgroup analysis
Motivated by visual analysis of Fig. 2, we tested for variance homogeneity across the experimental group. We found evidence of heterogeneous variances across groups (all Fs(1, 74) ≥ 8.62, ps ≤ 0.004) for several gaze parameters with larger variance within autistic individuals. The 99% confidence interval for the dwell-time variable on the eyes of the neurotypical group CI [23.04, 38.30] included 17 autistic individuals, whereas only 20 individuals from this group exhibited eye gaze below the lower threshold of that interval (note that a two-step cluster analysis revealed exactly the same group assignment; for details see Supplementary Results). These findings suggest the existence of at least two subgroups within our sample of autistic individuals, one group showing gaze behavior indistinguishable from that of neurotypical individuals, and one subgroup revealing reduced eye gaze (for details, see Tables S19, S20. S21 and S22).
In the next step, we checked whether we could find any variables (psychometric measures, clinical instruments, and subjective ratings) differing across these two subgroups of autistic individuals (one “high autism” group with values within the CI from neurotypical individuals, and another “low autism” group with values below the lower CI threshold). Surprisingly, among all state, trait, subjective, and objective rating parameters, we observed that groups differed only in the perceived “shame”, with higher shame ratings in the “high autism” group (F(1, 74) = 5.41, p = 0.023, η2 = 0.013). For all other variables there was no difference between groups (all F(1, 74) ≤ 1.72, p ≥ 0.143, η2 ≤ 0.009; Bayes Factors ranged from 1.48 to 3.12 suggesting that the likelihood of the alternative hypothesis – stating a group difference—is only slightly, i.e. 1.48 to 3.12 higher than that of the null hypothesis; for more details see Table S19, S20, S21, and 22). This seems to indicate that there may be two subgroups of autistic individuals distinguishable only in their gaze behavior, but not in other parameters.
Discussion
Relying on a novel dual eye-tracking setup, the present study compared the interactive dynamics of gaze behavior of autistic individuals to that of an age- and IQ-matched sample of neurotypical individuals during a naturalistic social encounter with a confederate (i.e., the FFP). As intended, the confederates revealed standardized gaze behavior that did not differ across groups. In contrast, we observed specific atypicalities in the interactive dynamics of gaze behavior among autistic individuals. Compared to neurotypical individuals, autistic individuals revealed fewer initiations and more break-ups of mutual eye gaze (i.e., eye contact), resulting in reduced eye contact with the confederate. Additional analyses suggested that reduced eye contact was mainly because autistic individuals broke-up eye contact more often. All differences across groups were more pronounced when participants were listening rather than talking during the social encounter, suggesting specific communication changes in autism. Autistic individuals also experienced the conversation as more aversive than neurotypical individuals. Overall, this study reveals several atypicalities in interactive gaze behavior in autism. Given the key role of eye contact in coordinating attention, understanding the other and exchanging information in social encounters (for example by signaling passes of the conversational baton; [68,69,70]), our findings might yield novel insights into the psychological mechanisms underlying altered social interaction associated with autism.
Specifically, our study reveals that reduced eye contact in interactions involving autistic individuals was mainly driven by more frequent eye contact break-ups by these individuals. Analyses of subjective ratings suggested that autistic individuals were less able or willing to maintain mutual eye gaze because they experience this state as somewhat aversive. These findings could be interpreted as favoring the “gaze avoidance account”, explaining atypical gaze behavior in autism by the attempt to minimize gaze-related hyperarousal [16,17,18,19]. Future studies including physiological measures of arousal (e.g., heart rate, [71, 72]) or even experimentally manipulating a situation’s aversiveness could well reinforce this assumption. On the other hand, we also observed that autistic individuals initiated eye contact less often, although this effect contributed less to the overall reduced eye contact. This finding also provides some evidence of the validity of the “gaze indifference account” explaining atypical gaze behavior by the inability or unwillingness to focus on the eyes as a source of social information [20,21,22]. It is interesting to relate these findings to another study applying dual eye-tracking during social observation via videoconference, which demonstrated that two interacting individuals showing some autistic characteristics revealed decreased eye gaze only when the other individual was looking back (but more eye gaze when the other individual was not looking back; [73]). They argue that these findings also provide evidence favoring the “gaze avoidance account” rather than the “gaze indifference account”, as eye gaze should not depend on the other individual’s eye gaze if individuals are indifferent to eye gaze. Overall, the existing dual eye-tracking studies on the association of autism and eye gaze suggest that the eye contact avoidance shown by autistic individuals is at least partly due to the fact that they experience eye contact as aversive.
Additional mechanistic insights into gaze behavior in autism are provided by comparing eye gaze across listening and speaking phases. Autistic individuals displayed larger atypicalities in the listener role. As in previous studies [68, 69] we found that, in general, individuals show more eye gaze when listening rather than speaking. This pattern suggests that gazing towards the interaction partner’s eyes while listening seems to benefit functional communication, for example by reinforcing one’s attention or by receiving additional information from the speaker (e.g., when have they finished and when am I expected to take over, emphasizing essential parts of communication by making eye contact while speaking; [68, 69]). In contrast, establishing eye gaze while speaking may be somewhat limited in all individuals, as it requires additional cognitive demands that might instead be channelled to optimize one’s own communication [2]. The observation that atypicalities in autistic individuals are more pronounced when listening might indicate that they have fewer opportunities to both signal their own attention to the other as well as receive rich social information that facilitates understanding of their interaction partner. It also raises the question of whether autistic individuals need more support during multimodal processing to process social information across various channels during face-to-face interactions [74].
Besides providing evidence in favor of differential gaze behavior across the groups of autistics individuals and neurotypical individuals, the present study also directs our attention to considerable heterogeneity within the sample of autistic individuals. Strikingly, our subgroup analysis revealed that only one specific autism subgroup exhibited atypical gaze behavior, constituting approximately half of the sample of autistic individuals. In contrast, another autism subgroup’s gaze behavior was barely distinguishable from that of neurotypical individuals. Except for revealing higher ratings for shame, the autism subgroup that exhibited atypical gaze behavior did not differ from the autism subgroup that exhibited typical gaze behavior in various state and trait parameters (e.g., subjective ratings, measures of neurodivergence, ADOS data). This pattern is reminiscent of findings from screen-based, non-interactive eye-tracking [75] and neuroimaging studies [76] that also identified behavioral and biological atypicalities only in a subgroup of autistic individuals. Such atypicalities might reflect endophenotypes reflecting genetic effects [77], potentially serving as biomarkers for use in identifying autistic characteristics and providing support to autistic individuals [27, 78, 79]. Indeed, assessing interactive gaze behavior via the present dual eye-tracking setup has potential translational significance. For example, in the future one could build a database of normative interactive gaze behavior to see whether an individual reveals atypicalities (i.e., scores outside the 95% confidence interval of the sample population) in different aspects of interactive gaze behavior (i.e., initiating eye-contact, breaking-up eye contact) as well as underlying physiological responses (i.e., heart rate). Such findings could reveal additional diagnostic information and tailor support of autistic individuals [80, 81] by indicating which individuals might benefit from support modules focused on psychotherapeutically modulating specific aspects of eye contact [82, 83], which individuals might benefit from reducing their autonomic hyperarousal during social interaction [84, 85], and which individuals might benefit from changing cognitive distortions regarding their otherwise typical social behavior (e.g., “I am unable to make adequate eye contact”).
Limitations
Whereas our study has taken an initial approach to analyze the interactive dynamics of gaze behavior based on event classification, our data might offer even more to discover when taking more complex analysis approaches (e.g., time series analysis; [86]). In addition, it would be insightful to add arousal-related physiological measures (e.g., heart rate or electrodermal activity) to check whether the typical interactive gaze behavior displayed by a subgroup of autistic individuals comes at a cost to them (i.e., accompanied by increased arousal). It is also an interesting question whether autistic individuals would show the same pattern of gaze behavior when interacting with other autistic individuals. According to the theory of the “double empathy problem” [87, 88], neurotypical individuals have difficulty understanding autistic individuals, meaning that the atypical gaze behavior of autistic individuals could also be driven by the “atypical” gaze behavior of neurotypical individuals. Future research should also include female autistic individuals to test the generalizability of the present study’s findings across genders. Finally, although our setup takes a large step forward in studying naturalistic, interactive social behavior under controlled experimental conditions, it remains a target of investigation as to whether the gaze behavior displayed within this setup generalizes to real-world gaze behavior, given that both the setup itself might still make participants aware of the test situation and the interaction was semi-structured.
Conclusion
In sum, by exploiting analyses of dual eye-tracking data, the present study has identified specific atypicalities in the interactive dynamics of gaze behavior in autistic individuals' naturalistic face-to-face interactions. By identifying subgroups with either typical or atypical gaze behavior, we might be able to develop more individualized diagnostic and support approaches in the future. Furthermore, the present study raises issues ideal for future research. Are there any other psychological or biological parameters distinguishing gaze behavior subgroups? Do autistic individuals also exhibit altered interactive gaze behavior dynamics in social situations involving explicitly negative social evaluations or conflict [89]? Could administering oxytocin modulate interactive gaze behavior in autistic individuals who do not initiate but rather break-up eye contact [11, 90,91,92]? Overall, given the extensive significance of eye contact in enabling functional face-to-face social interactions in both neurotypical and neurodiverse individuals, this study’s minimally invasive dual eye-tracking setup could prove to be of great value for researchers across the social, clinical, and behavioral sciences.
Availability of data and materials
Data and materials are available on request.
References
Risko EF, Richardson DC, Kingstone A. Breaking the fourth wall of cognitive science: real-world social attention and the dual function of gaze. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2016;25:70–4.
Ho S, Foulsham T, Kingstone A. Speaking and listening with the eyes: gaze signaling during dyadic interactions. PLoS ONE. 2015;10: e0136905.
Redcay E, Schilbach L. Using second-person neuroscience to elucidate the mechanisms of social interaction. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2019;20:495–505.
Lehmann K, Bolis D, Friston KJ, Schilbach L, Ramstead MJD, Kanske P. An active-inference approach to second-person neuroscience. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2023;19(6):931–51.
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. 5th edition. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.
Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, et al. The autism diagnostic observation schedule—generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30:205–23.
Black MH, Chen NTM, Iyer KK, Lipp OV, Bölte S, Falkmer M, et al. Mechanisms of facial emotion recognition in autism spectrum disorders: Insights from eye tracking and electroencephalography. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;80:488–515.
Setien-Ramos I, Lugo-Marín J, Gisbert-Gustemps L, Díez-Villoria E, Magán-Maganto M, Canal-Bedia R, et al. Eye-tracking studies in adults with autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Autism Dev Disord. 2023;53:2430–43.
Frazier TW, Strauss M, Klingemier EW, Zetzer EE, Hardan AY, Eng C, et al. A meta-analysis of gaze differences to social and nonsocial information between individuals with and without autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;56:546–55.
Riddiford JA, Enticott PG, Lavale A, Gurvich C. Gaze and social functioning associations in autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Autism Res. 2022;15:1380–446.
Auyeung B, Lombardo MV, Heinrichs M, Chakrabarti B, Sule A, Deakin JB, et al. Oxytocin increases eye contact during a real-time, naturalistic social interaction in males with and without autism. Transl Psychiatry. 2015;5: e507.
Nyström P, Thorup E, Bölte S, Falck-Ytter T. Joint attention in infancy and the emergence of autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2019;86:631–8.
Thorup E, Nyström P, Gredebäck G, Bölte S, Falck-Ytter T. Altered gaze following during live interaction in infants at risk for autism: an eye tracking study. Mol Autism. 2016;7:12. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13229-016-0069-9.
Falck-Ytter T. Gaze performance during face-to-face communication: a live eye tracking study of typical children and children with autism. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2015;17:78–85.
Tönsing D, Schiller B, Vehlen A, Spenthof I, Domes G, Heinrichs M. No evidence that gaze anxiety predicts gaze avoidance behavior during face-to-face social interaction. Sci Rep. 2022;12:21332.
Dalton KM, Nacewicz BM, Johnstone T, Schaefer HS, Gernsbacher MA, Goldsmith HH, et al. Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:519–26.
Tanaka JW, Sung A. The, “eye avoidance” hypothesis of autism face processing. J Autism Dev Disord. 2016;46:1538–52.
Kliemann D, Dziobek I, Hatri A, Steimke R, Heekeren HR. Atypical reflexive gaze patterns on emotional faces in autism spectrum disorders. J Neurosci. 2010;30:12281–7.
Stuart N, Whitehouse A, Palermo R, Bothe E, Badcock N. Eye gaze in autism spectrum disorder: a review of neural evidence for the eye avoidance hypothesis. J Autism Dev Disord. 2023;53:1884–905.
Moriuchi JM, Klin A, Jones W. Mechanisms of diminished attention to eyes in autism. Am J Psychiatry. 2017;174:26–35.
Senju A, Johnson MH. The eye contact effect: mechanisms and development. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13:127–34.
Baron-Cohen S, Ring HA, Bullmore ET, Wheelwright S, Ashwin C, Williams SC. The amygdala theory of autism. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2000;24:355–64.
Laidlaw KEW, Kingstone A. Fixations to the eyes aids in facial encoding; covertly attending to the eyes does not. Acta Psychol. 2017;173:55–65.
Thompson SJ, Foulsham T, Leekam SR, Jones CRG. Attention to the face is characterised by a difficult to inhibit first fixation to the eyes. Acta Psychol. 2019;193:229–38.
Vehlen A, Spenthof I, Tönsing D, Heinrichs M, Domes G. Evaluation of an eye tracking setup for studying visual attention in face-to-face conversations. Sci Rep. 2021;11:2661.
Aron A, Melinat E, Aron EN, Vallone RD, Bator RJ. The experimental generation of interpersonal closeness: a procedure and some preliminary findings. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 1997;23:363–77.
Jensen AR, Lane AL, Werner BA, McLees SE, Fletcher TS, Frye RE. Modern biomarkers for autism spectrum disorder: future directions. Mol Diagn Ther. 2022;26:483–95.
Loomes R, Hull L, Mandy WPL. What is the male-to-female ratio in autism spectrum disorder? A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;56:466–74.
Napolitano A, Schiavi S, La Rosa P, Rossi-Espagnet MC, Petrillo S, Bottino F, et al. Sex differences in autism spectrum disorder: diagnostic, neurobiological, and behavioral features. Front Psychiatry. 2022;13: 889636.
Zhang Y, Li N, Li C, Zhang Z, Teng H, Wang Y, et al. Genetic evidence of gender difference in autism spectrum disorder supports the female-protective effect. Transl Psychiatry. 2020;10:4.
Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39:175–91.
Brustkern J, Heinrichs M, Walker M, Schiller B. Facial threat affects trust more strongly than facial attractiveness in women than it does in men. Sci Rep. 2021;11:22475.
Schiller B, Brustkern J, Walker M, Hamm A, Heinrichs M. Oxytocin has sex-specific effects on trust and underlying neurophysiological processes. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2023;151: 106076.
Franke GH, Jaeger S, Glaesmer H, Barkmann C, Petrowski K, Braehler E. Psychometric analysis of the brief symptom inventory 18 (BSI-18) in a representative German sample. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2017;17:14.
Derogatis LR. Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI). APA PsycTests. 1982. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/t00789-000.
Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Beck-Depressions-Inventar (BDI-II, dt. Version). M, Hautzinger, F, Keller, C Kühner, 2. 2009.
Wintjen L, Petermann F. Beck-Depressions-Inventar Revision (BDI–II). Z fur Psychiatr Psychol Psychother. 2010;58:243–5.
Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E. The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31:5–17.
Freitag CM, Retz-Junginger P, Retz W, Seitz C, Palmason H, Meyer J, et al. German adaptation of the autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evaluation and short version AQ-k. Z fur Psychiatr Psychol Psychother. 2007;36:280–9.
Davis MH. Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1983;44:113–26.
Paulus C. The Saarbrueck Personality Questionnaire on empathy: psychometric evaluation of the German version of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index [Internet]. 2009. Available from: https://psycharchives.org/en/item/2b090feb-4f9e-4ea2-9524-13151172f615
Mattick RP, Clarke JC. Development and validation of measures of social phobia scrutiny fear and social interaction anxiety. Behav Res Ther. 1998;36:455–70.
Stangier U, Heidenreich T, Berardi A, Golbs U, Hoyer J. Assessment of social phobia by the social interaction anxiety scale (SIAS) and the Social Phobia Scale (SPS)]. Z Klein Psychol Psychother. 1999;28:28–36.
Schneier FR, Rodebaugh TL, Blanco C, Lewin H, Liebowitz MR. Fear and avoidance of eye contact in social anxiety disorder. Compr Psychiatry. 2011;52:81–7.
Domes G, Marx L, Spenthof I, Heinrichs M. The German version of the gaze anxiety rating scale (GARS): reliability and validity. PLoS ONE. 2016;11: e0150807.
Leary MR. A brief version of the fear of negative evaluation scale. Pers Soc Psychol B. 1983;9:371–5.
Reichenberger J, Schwarz M, König D, Wilhelm FH, Voderholzer U, Hillert A, et al. Translation of the brief fear of negative evaluation scale-revised (BFNE-R) and validation of the German version (FNE-K). Diagnostica. 2016;62:169–81.
Lord C, Jones RM. Annual research review: re-thinking the classification of autism spectrum disorders. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53:490–509.
Poustka L, Rühl D, Feineis-Matthews S, Bölte S, Poustka F, Hartung M. ADOS-2. Diagnostische Beobachtungsskala für Autistische Störungen - 2. 2015.
First MB, Williams JBW, Karg RS, Spitzer RL. User’s guide for the SCID-5-CV Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5® disorders: clinical version. Arlington, VA, US: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.; 2016. p. xii, 158.
First MB, Beesdo-Baum K, Zaudig M, Wittchen HU. SCID-5-PD: structured clinical interview for DSM-5 personality disorders – German version. DSM-5® - personality disorders by Michael B. First, Janet B.W. Wiliams, Lorna Smith Benjamin, Robert L. Spitzer : Manual [Internet]. Hogrefe; 2019. Available from: https://books.google.de/books?id=8pDbxQEACAAJ
Cattell RB. Culture Fair Intelligence Test (CFIT) [Database record]. APA PsycTests. 1940;
Weiß R. Culture Fair Intelligence Test 20-R—Scale 2. 2006.
Lehrl S, Triebig G, Fischer B. Multiple choice vocabulary test MWT as a valid and short test to estimate premorbid intelligence. Acta Neurol Scand. 1995;91:335–45.
Aron A, Aron EN, Smollan D. Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1992;63:596–612.
Amos B, Ludwiczuk B, Satyanarayanan M. Openface: a general-purpose face recognition library with mobile applications. CMU Sch Comput Sci. 2016;6:1–20.
Hessels RS, Kemner C, van den Boomen C, Hooge ITC. The area-of-interest problem in eyetracking research: a noise-robust solution for face and sparse stimuli. Behav Res Methods. 2016;48:1694–712.
Vehlen A, Standard W, Domes G. How to choose the size of facial areas of interest in interactive eye tracking. PLoS ONE. 2022;17: e0263594.
Thompson PD, Colebatch JG, Brown P, Rothwell JC, Day BL, Obeso JA, et al. Voluntary stimulus-sensitive jerks and jumps mimicking myoclonus or pathological startle syndromes. Mov Disord. 1992;7:257–62.
Jain A, Bansal R, Kumar A, Singh KD. A comparative study of visual and auditory reaction times on the basis of gender and physical activity levels of medical first year students. Int J Appl Basic Med Res. 2015;5:124–7.
Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Package Lme4: linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version. 2014.
Akaike H. Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika. 1987;52:317–32.
Burnham KP, Anderson DR. Multimodel inference: understanding aic and bic in model selection. Sociol Methods Res. 2004;33:261–304.
Barr DJ. Random effects structure for testing interactions in linear mixed-effects models. Front Psychol. 2013;4:328.
Blanca Mena MJ, Alarcón Postigo R, Arnau Gras J, Bono Cabré R, Bendayan R. Non-normal data: is ANOVA still a valid option? Psicothema. 2017;29:552–7.
Jeffreys H. Theory of probability. 3rd edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press;
Rouder JN, Speckman PL, Sun D, Morey RD, Iverson G. Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychon Bull Rev. 2009;16:225–37.
Wohltjen S, Wheatley T. Eye contact marks the rise and fall of shared attention in conversation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2021;118: e2106645118.
Templeton EM, Chang LJ, Reynolds EA, Cone LeBeaumont MD, Wheatley T. Fast response times signal social connection in conversation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2022;119: e2116915119.
Degutyte Z, Astell A. The role of eye gaze in regulating turn taking in conversations: a systematized review of methods and findings. Front Psychol. 2021;12: 616471.
Rösler L, Göhring S, Strunz M, Gamer M. Social anxiety is associated with heart rate but not gaze behavior in a real social interaction. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2021;70: 101600.
Wieser MJ, Pauli P, Alpers GW, Mühlberger A. Is eye to eye contact really threatening and avoided in social anxiety?–An eye-tracking and psychophysiology study. J Anxiety Disord. 2009;23:93–103.
Hessels RS, Holleman GA, Cornelissen THW, Hooge ITC, Kemner C. Eye contact takes two – autistic and social anxiety traits predict gaze behavior in dyadic interaction. J Exp Psychopathol. 2018;9: 062917.
Benetti S, Ferrari A, Pavani F. Multimodal processing in face-to-face interactions: a bridging link between psycholinguistics and sensory neuroscience. Front Hum Neurosci. 2023;17:1108354.
Keles U, Kliemann D, Byrge L, Saarimäki H, Paul LK, Kennedy DP, et al. Atypical gaze patterns in autistic adults are heterogeneous across but reliable within individuals. Mol Autism. 2022;13:39.
Byrge L, Dubois J, Tyszka JM, Adolphs R, Kennedy DP. Idiosyncratic brain activation patterns are associated with poor social comprehension in autism. J Neurosci. 2015;35:5837–50.
Constantino JN, Kennon-McGill S, Weichselbaum C, Marrus N, Haider A, Glowinski AL, et al. Infant viewing of social scenes is under genetic control and is atypical in autism. Nature. 2017;547:340–4.
Shic F, Naples AJ, Barney EC, Chang SA, Li B, McAllister T, et al. The autism biomarkers consortium for clinical trials: evaluation of a battery of candidate eye-tracking biomarkers for use in autism clinical trials. Mol Autism. 2022;13:15.
Parellada M, Andreu-Bernabeu Á, Burdeus M, San José Cáceres A, Urbiola E, Carpenter LL, et al. In search of biomarkers to guide interventions in autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review. Am J Psychiatry. 2023;180:23–40.
Ahmed IA, Senan EM, Rassem TH, Ali MAH, Shatnawi HSA, Alwazer SM, Alshahrani M. Eye tracking-based diagnosis and early detection of autism spectrum disorder using machine learning and deep learning techniques. Electronics. 2022;11(4):530.
Greene RK, Parish-Morris J, Sullivan M, Kinard JL, Mosner MG, Turner-Brown LM, et al. Dynamic eye tracking as a predictor and outcome measure of social skills intervention in adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2021;51:1173–87.
Miltenberger RG, Valbuena D, Sanchez S. Applied behavior analysis. Handbook of cognitive behavioral therapy: overview and approaches, Vol 1. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association; 2021. p. 637–71.
Tebartz van Elst L, Fangmeier T, Schaller UM, Hennig O, Kieser M, Koelkebeck K, et al. FASTER and SCOTT&EVA trainings for adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder (ASD): study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials. 2021;22:261.
Patriquin MA, Hartwig EM, Friedman BH, Porges SW, Scarpa A. Autonomic response in autism spectrum disorder: relationship to social and cognitive functioning. Biol Psychol. 2019;145:185–97.
Aishworiya R, Valica T, Hagerman R, Restrepo B. An update on psychopharmacological treatment of autism spectrum disorder. Neurother. 2022;19:248–62.
Coco MI, Dale R. Cross-recurrence quantification analysis of categorical and continuous time series: an R package. Front Psychol. 2014;5:510.
Milton DEM. On the ontological status of autism: the ‘double empathy problem.’ Disability & Society. 2012;27:883–7.
Mitchell P, Sheppard E, Cassidy S. Autism and the double empathy problem: implications for development and mental health. Br J Dev Psychol. 2021;39:1–18.
Langer JK, Lim MH, Fernandez KC, Rodebaugh TL. Social anxiety disorder is associated with reduced eye contact during conversation primed for conflict. Cognit Ther Res. 2017;41:220–9.
Le J, Kou J, Zhao W, Fu M, Zhang Y, Becker B, et al. Oxytocin biases eye-gaze to dynamic and static social images and the eyes of fearful faces: associations with trait autism. Transl Psychiatry. 2020;10:142.
Kanat M, Spenthof I, Riedel A, van Elst LT, Heinrichs M, Domes G. Restoring effects of oxytocin on the attentional preference for faces in autism. Transl Psychiatry. 2017;7:e1097–e1097.
Vehlen A, Kellner A, Normann C, Heinrichs M, Domes G. Reduced eye gaze during facial emotion recognition in chronic depression: effects of intranasal oxytocin. J Psychiatr Res. 2023;159:50–6.
Bottema-Beutel K, Kapp SK, Lester JN, Sasson NJ, Hand BN. Avoiding ableist language: suggestions for autism researchers. Autism Adulthood. 2021;3(1):18–29.
Acknowledgements
We thank Antonia Blunk, Caroline Fierle, Theresa Tropf, Adam Bauer, Lukas Königs, Niklas Kaiser, Simon Kieffer, Niklas Hohagen, Alex Lessing, Steve Calcada, Sven Schaal, for assistance during data collection, and Lars Nitzke for his valuable support during programming work.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. This research was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, Grant No. HE5310/5–1 to MH and DO1312/5–1 to GD). Bastian Schiller acknowledges support from the European Research Foundation (ERC Starting Grant SODI, “From face-to-face to face-to-screen: Social animals interacting in a digital world, Project 101076414). Kathrin Nickel acknowledges support from the Berta-Ottenstein-Programme for Advanced Clinician Scientists, Faculty of Medicine, University of Freiburg).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
M.H., G.D., A.V., and D.T. designed the study, B.S., D.T., K.N., L.TvE., and M.H. organized participant recruitment and data collection, D.T. performed data collection and pre-processing, D.T, G.D. and A.V. conceptualized the analyses scripts, D.T., B.S., M.H., G.D. and A.V. analysed the data. G.D., and M.H. acquired funding for the study. D.T. and B.S. wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to interpreting the results and revised the manuscript. All authors have approved the final version of this article.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
The ethics committee of the University of Freiburg approved this study (reference number: EK-Freiburg: 439/15). All participants gave written informed consent before the experiment took place.
Autism-related language preferences
The authors of this paper aimed to use terminology that aligns with the language preferences of autistic adults, referring to [93].
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Tönsing, D., Schiller, B., Vehlen, A. et al. Altered interactive dynamics of gaze behavior during face-to-face interaction in autistic individuals: a dual eye-tracking study. Molecular Autism 16, 12 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13229-025-00645-5
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s13229-025-00645-5